Southern New Hampshire University Research Methods Discussion

For this assignment you will refer to Theoretical Research in the 

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

Handbook of Social Work Research Methods

Download Handbook of Social Work Research Methods

.

Define theoretical research.

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

What is the distinction between basic and applied empirical research?

  • What is meant by the term ‘theory’ in this chapter?
  • What is meant by the four types of theories described on pages 470-471?

    What are theories made of?  List these elements and provide an original example of each one.

    Describe any two characteristics of a good theory.

    Describe any two sources of social work theory.

    Distinguish between theoretical exegesis and theoretical criticism.

  • Write one thoughtful question or comment about this chapter.
  • Is there any theory relevant to the topic you have been writing out?  Why or why not?
  • Locate  and provide APA style citations for theories related to your topic. For  this assignment you can cite three papers about one theory or three  addressing 2-3 different theories. Remember to explain why you think the  theory or theories relate to your topic. You could select a theory that  is psychosocial in nature, or biological (e.g., the dopamine  hypothesis), or even something else. Try and draw upon pertinent  theories regardless of the discipline the theory comes from.
  • Assignment 2 Part 1 (B)
  • Draft three thoughtful questions or comments about the article What is the Role of Theory in Research on Social Work Practice?
  • Explain the role of formal theory in your own practice.
  • Assignment 2 Part 2
  • Prepare a draft literature review on the topic of your ACP.
  • Below  is guidance for you as you write your draft. Remember to revise this  draft at least once before submitting to your instructor.
  • Often  literature reviews make the most sense when organized chronologically,  beginning with early work and then progressing to more contemporary  resources.
  • Empirically-based  peer reviewed journal articles will typically form the majority of your  citations, but these will not be exclusively the basis of your  literature review.
  • In the  beginning of the draft, describe the search process you used to locate  the literature you cited.  This should be done in a manner that permits  the reader to replicate your search process using the same method and  obtain the same results.

    Refer to the multimedia activity, Block it Out, for more details and guidance on how to organize your literature review.

    PART V
    C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    467
    CHAPTER 26
    Theoretical Research
    Bruce A. Thyer
    Becoming a theorist is not a mysterious process. It works in much the same way as
    becoming a researcher or teacher—through formal and informal learning experiences.
    —Randolph-Seng (2008, p. 39)
    eretofore, the chapters in this handbook have been concerned with different research methods to systematically gather empirical data for various purposes. Sometimes the purposes of empirical research are
    exploratory in nature—to learn about some phenomenon of interest, to
    become more informed about it, or to use it as a preliminary for additional work. On other occasions, we gather data for purely descriptive
    purposes, to be able to quantify variables of interest or to learn more about the subjective
    experiences of people such as social work clients. Much social work research is explanatory
    in nature, intended to help us understand linkages and relationships among variables and
    sometimes to try and develop causal knowledge about the etiology of psychosocial problems. And another useful type of research is the development and testing of methods of
    social work assessment such as rating scales, rapid assessment instruments, and indices.
    The least common but perhaps most valuable type of social work research are evaluation
    studies, actually looking at the processes and outcomes of social work services, programs,
    and policies. Whether the research method involves quantitative measurement or qualitative data or uses a mixed methods approach, thus far we have been discussing methods
    involving the gathering of empirical data, with such studies being generally lumped under
    the term of empirical research. Empirical research itself may be divided into two types, basic
    empirical research (which is, roughly speaking, aimed at developing an understanding of
    psychosocial phenomena) and applied empirical research (aimed at solving problems).
    Social work tends to involve very little basic empirical research, which is understandable given our profession’s focus on solving serious psychosocial programs, a focus almost
    synonymous with the term applied. We are a practice profession, not an academic discipline studying things for their own sake. The public expects us to develop effective solutions to problems, and we study theory primarily to advance that purpose. Turner (1979)
    expressed this view in the following statement:
    H
    The clinician’s principal interest is in the utility of theory: what can it tell me about
    this situation that will permit me to act effectively? It is, therefore, not knowledge for
    its own sake, but knowledge for use. (p. 3, italics added)
    468
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    469
    This is a pragmatic view similar to that described about two decades earlier:
    Social work is not a science whose aim is to derive knowledge; it is a technology
    whose aim is to apply knowledge for the purpose of control. Therefore, on the
    research continuum social work research falls nearer to the applied end, because its
    purpose is practice knowledge. (Greenwood, 1957, p. 315)
    and
    A social worker in his capacity as a social worker is not a social scientist. His role as
    a practitioner is not primarily composed of observing, understanding, doing
    research, and developing theory. As a practitioner he must act, and act with whatever tools and knowledge he has at his command. . . . The primary value of social
    science content for social work . . . is its relevance to practice. The ultimate test of
    such relevance is what difference the content can make in social work’s capacity to
    help people. (Stein, 1960, p. 200)
    The present chapter is devoted to a topic different from either basic or applied
    empirical research, a field of scholarship labeled theoretical research, which,
    for the purposes of this discussion, refers to original explanatory formulations
    advanced without the benefit of systematic and originally collected empirical data.
    Randolph-Seng (2008, p. 39) provides a similar description: “A theorist examines a
    systematic collection of evidence to create ‘intellectual structures’ that make sense of
    diverse findings and, in turn, provide distinct predictions that can be examined
    empirically.”
    Table 26.1 presents some common definitions of theory derived from the social work
    literature presented over the years, definitions that represent the sense in which the term
    is used in this chapter. Other conceptions can be presented as to what is meant by the
    word theory, but the usage of the term in this chapter is the most commonly understood
    and accepted meaning of the word.
    Social work scholarship focusing on theoretical research of any type is rare. Daley,
    Peters, Taylor, Hanson, and Hills (2006) examined 885 articles retrieved from 30 social
    work journals published in 2002 and categorized them as to the type of research that
    was represented. More than 41% of the articles were empirical studies with no theoretical linkages, and 30.7% were articles without any empirical or theoretical foundations
    (e.g., about 71% of the articles contained no theory discussion at all). Few articles
    (9.5%) contained any content representing the advancement of theorizing. Lest the
    reader think that the year 2002 was an aberration, Gentle-Genitty et al. (2007) partially
    replicated Daley et al. (2006) by retrieving and examining 1,168 articles published during 2004 that were obtained from 37 social work journals. Only 6% (n = 71) met the criteria for contributing to theory development with an empirical base, and fully 12 of the
    journals (32%) did not contain any articles involving theory development and explanation. These authors concluded that “overall, the results indicate minimal focus on
    theory within the 37 journals selected for review” (Gentle-Genitty et al., 2007, pp. 69–70).
    Given the crucial importance that is ascribed to good theories within social work
    (Thyer, 2001a, 2001b, 2008c), the relative lack of attention to theoretical developments
    within our field is rather shocking.
    470 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    T A B L E 26.1
    Some Selected Definitions of the Term Theory
    • “Theory consists of an interlocking set of hypotheses that are logically related, and it seeks to explain the
    inter-relationships among empirical generalizations” (Tripodi, Fellin, & Meyer, 1969, p. 13).
    • “Theory in social work is intended to provide explanations for the phenomena of practice” (Lewis, 1971,
    p. 16).
    • “For the clinician seeking to offer responsible, effective, intervention, the essential and important
    contribution of theory is its ability to predict outcomes, or, in other words, its ability to explain” (Turner,
    1979, p. 8).
    • “Theories are sets of concepts and constructs that describe and explain natural phenomena” (Tolson,
    Reid, & Garvin, 1994, p. 241).
    • A “collection of related statements or propositions that attempt to describe, explain, or predict a
    particular aspect of experience” (Queralt, 1996, p. 12).
    • “A group of related hypotheses, concepts and constructs, based on facts and observations, that
    attempts to explain a particular phenomenon.” (Barker, 2003, p. 434).
    • “The essential idea of theory is that it is assumptions, that it can systematise, and that it can explain
    phenomena” (Johnsson & Svensson, 2005, p. 420).
    • “A systematic set of interrelated statements intended to explain some aspect of social life or enrich our
    sense of how people conduct and find meaning in their daily lives” (Rubin & Babbie, 2008, p. 646).
    • “A reasoned set of propositions, derived from and supported by established data, which serves to
    explain a group of phenomena” (Grinnell & Unrau, 2008, p. 558).
    • “A more or less well argued explanation of reality. Collectively, then, theories are explanations of
    reality—of human behaviour or particular social phenomena” (Gray & Webb, 2009, p. 10).
    NOTE: Italics added in all definitions.
    Types of Theories
    Theories themselves can be roughly categorized into those focusing on description, explanation, prediction (e.g., creating hypotheses), and prescription (e.g., interventive theory),
    with grand theories encompassing all four of these aspects and being applicable across a
    wide range of social work practice and psychosocial phenomena. Some examples of grand
    theories include psychoanalytic theory and its numerous psychodynamic offshoots; social
    learning theory, comprising operant, respondent, and observational learning principles;
    general systems theory and its social work variants, such as the ecosystems perspective; and
    so forth. Generally, grand theories provide conceptual frameworks to explain normal
    human development across the life span, the development of dysfunctional behavior
    across the life span, and behavior occurring at the level of individuals, families, small
    groups, organizations, communities, and society as a whole. Obviously, relatively few grand
    theories fulfill these ambitious goals, and those that do possess such wide applicability generally do so in theory only, so to speak, not in empirically supported applications. In the
    fullness of time, it is hoped that the social and behavioral sciences will move toward a grand
    unified theory of human behavior embracing actions at all levels of analysis: individual,
    couples, families, small groups, organizations, communities, nations, and larger units of
    analysis. This would be an idealized goal analogous to the search for a grand unified theory
    (a.k.a. GUT) within the discipline of physics, one that mathematically unites the principles
    of gravity, strong and weak forces within the atom, and electromagnetism. Although we are
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    471
    obviously a very long way from having such a theory of everything in social work, there is
    a long tradition of sober-minded thinkers who have explored the possibility of this within
    the social and behavioral sciences and even an eventual unification of the behavioral and
    physical sciences (see Causey, 1977). Others deny the theoretical possibility of having a
    theory of everything, but then heavier than air travel was once deemed impossible also.
    Absent satisfactory grand unified theories or less large-scale theories, we are often content to contend with a lesser level of explanation, called middle-range theories, which are
    more limited in scope and address a circumscribed area or domain of practice (see Martin
    & Sugarman, 2009). These might address a particular client group (e.g., Afrocentric
    theory or adolescent development), the etiology of a particular problem (e.g., the tension
    reduction theory of alcohol abuse, the stress-diathesis theory of schizophrenia), or the
    presumptive mechanisms of action explaining how a specific intervention is supposed to
    work (the theory behind the principles of Alcoholics Anonymous or the theory behind
    cognitive therapy for depression). Obviously, there are far more middle-range theories
    than there are grand theories in social work, but it would be hoped that in the fullness of
    time and the accumulation of data that middle-range theories would be legitimately integrated into one or more grand theories. This level of theory has been described by the
    sociologist Robert K. Merton as follows:
    Merton asks sociologists to be more modest in their goals when developing theories.
    Instead of attempting to develop highly general, and therefore abstract, theories to
    explain the structure and functioning of total societies, he proposes a series of less general
    theories be developed to explain smaller components of social reality. Thus, Merton’s
    approach would have sociologists concentrate on developing theories of deviance, stratification, minority-majority relations, population change, and so forth that are less
    abstract but more closely tied to empirical reality, thus more testable. Only after a
    number of such theories are developed should attempts be made to move up the level of
    abstraction and develop more general, all encompassing theories. (Chafetz, 1978, p. 6)
    Lesser still in scope are so-called low-level or practice theories, theories arising to
    account for highly specific situations. While appealing in many ways—they can make
    intuitive sense, may require lesser levels of abstraction, and can be developed for most
    situations—low-level theories threaten to overwhelm the profession with a plethora of
    competing perspectives. Loeb (1960) referred to them in this way:
    Low level, or ad hoc, theories are annoying. They fill the literature with research
    reports of little significance or relevance. On the other hand, a few of them have
    important effects in modifying middle-range theories. These pearls may be worth
    the task of opening all the oysters. (Loeb, 1960, p. 8)
    Theoretical advances are very highly valued within the world of academics and often
    seen as a somewhat mystical process, particularly by practitioners. Creating a new theory
    is seen as more prestigious than extending an established one, and as a consequence,
    sometimes existing theory is simply reframed in new language to give the appearance of
    having done something more novel than may actually be the case. The distinguished psychologist Walter Mischel (2009) made these observations on the subject:
    Our urban legends, and the reality that underlies them, directly reward research and
    theorizing that emphasizes novelty and newsworthiness in publishing, grant getting, and tenure requirements, especially in highly competitive universities. You get
    472 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    high marks for autonomy and brand new theories, unconnected to anybody else’s
    ideas, especially those of one’s mentors. That’s great, when the contribution is really
    new and solid, and adds to building a cumulative science. It’s not great when it turns
    out to be mostly recycled—repackaged with a more newsworthy sexy title . . . psychologists tend to treat other people’s theories like toothbrushes; no self-respecting
    individual wants to use anyone else’s. (p. 3)
    One highly respected approach to social work is called task-centered practice (TCP; see
    Reid, 1992). Unlike most other approaches, this model is one of the few that was conceptualized from within the profession, by and for social workers, to be applied to social work
    practice. TCP enjoys a solid empirical foundation (see the November 2008 special issue of
    the journal Research on Social Work Practice, focusing on this one approach) and has generated a modest literature in its own right. However, Gambrill (1994) critiqued TCP as
    being essentially (my words here) watered down behavior analysis and therapy, failing to
    sufficiently acknowledge the essential similarities between TCP and behavioral methods
    and understating the valuable role that social learning theory plays in explaining how certain methods of TCP assessment and intervention may work. Similarly, Thyer and Myers
    (1997) reviewed the essential similarities between the person-in-environment (PIE) perspective so widely accepted within social work and the person and environment interactionist learning theory foundations of behavioral methods (which considerably predated
    PIE in social work), noting,
    The parallels between the two are striking, so much so that it would seem that contemporary social workers are reinventing the wheel, woefully ignorant of the social
    learning theory antecedents of current models or are simply choosing to ignore the
    similarities between the two approaches. (p. 21)
    The psychotherapy called eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR)
    has been similarly critiqued as a weak form of cognitive behavioral therapy, and the originator of EMDR also has been frankly criticized as engaging in making exaggerated claims
    and hucksterism as a means of promoting this technique, at great personal financial
    profit. Attempting to read the minds of social work theorists is always a risky proposition,
    but the temptation to relabel existing theory and to pass it off as a new development, perhaps with new language and proposed causal mechanisms, is undoubtedly a real dynamic
    in the world of theoretical development. It adds conceptual confusion and violates the
    principle of parsimony discussed in Chapter 1 of this book.
    What Are Theories Made of?
    Philosophical and Conceptual Assumptions
    Theories can be said to comprise a variety of elements. At the highest level of abstraction,
    a theory can posit a general worldview or set of philosophical assumptions. Most theories
    in the social and behavioral sciences, including social work, implicitly or explicitly accept
    the philosophy of science called positivism, which holds “that social behavior could be
    studied and understood in a rational, scientific manner—in contrast to explanations
    based on religion or superstition” (Rubin & Babbie, 2008, p. 642). Although subjected to
    many differing interpretations and often misunderstood, the relatively benign definition
    of positivism provided above remains the mainstream perspective of social science (also
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    473
    see Thyer, 2008a). A contrasting view is called social constructivism, which maintains that
    much of our reality is socially constructed. The two views are not antithetical, of course,
    but they can serve as polar opposites anchoring our initial attempt at describing what
    goes into a theory’s umbrella perspectives. It should be acknowledged that these fundamental philosophical underpinnings of social work theory cannot themselves be subjected to scientific scrutiny. They are assumptions that various theories accept or reject,
    recognizing that these propositions cannot be proven to be correct (or incorrect). In this
    sense, one’s philosophical positions can be seen as similar to the beginning axioms with
    which Euclid developed his system of geometry. Accept these axioms, and a very elaborate
    theoretical construction becomes possible.
    Propositions
    The next level of abstraction can be called a theory’s essential propositions, general statements about the empirical world that is the focus of theory. To illustrate, here are a few
    examples of some fundamental propositions derived from psychodynamic theory:
    • A substantial part of the activity of the human mind consists of unconscious
    processes.
    • Unconscious processes are involved in cognition, affect, and motivation.
    • Important motivators of human activity are one’s sexual and aggressive urges.
    • People can feel things without knowing they feel them, and they can act on feelings
    of which they are unaware.
    These are just a few representative ones, by no means suggestive of the comprehensive
    picture of the richness of this conceptual framework (e.g., see Westen, 1998).
    Social learning theory has its own propositions, such as the following:
    • Human behavior consists of what we do—both observable behavior and unobservable behavior. Overt acts, covert speech, thoughts and cognition, feelings, dreams:
    all those phenomena people engage in are considered behavior.
    • To a large extent, much (but not all) human behavior is learned through life experiences. This learning occurs throughout the life span.
    • It seems very likely that similar fundamental learning processes give rise to individual behavior across cultures and life circumstances and accounts for both normative
    and many so-called dysfunctional actions, feelings, and thoughts.
    • Interpersonal behavior is also a function of these learning processes, giving rise to
    dyadic, group, organizational, community, and societal phenomena. These larger
    scale activities are, to a great extent, a more complex operation of fundamental
    learning mechanisms. (Thyer & Myers, 1997, p. 19)
    And cognitive theory has its own propositions as well, such as the following:
    • Individuals respond to cognitive representations of environmental events rather
    than to the events per se.
    • Learning is cognitively mediated.
    • At least some forms of cognition can be self-monitored.
    • At least some forms of cognition can be altered.
    • Altering cognition can change dysfunctional patterns of emotion and behavior.
    (Thyer & Myers, 1997, p. 29)
    474 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    It is always desirable for a scientifically sound theory to clearly outline its foundational
    propositions, and these should be clearly explicable in everyday language. From such
    propositions can be derived specific, predictive hypotheses, statements about what can be
    logically concluded to happen under certain circumstances within everyday life using naturalistic research methods or in the deliberately contrived or manipulated situations we
    call experiments or demonstrations. Clinical evidence of the validity of the above psychodynamic propositions is seen by some theorists through phenomena such as slips of the
    tongue, neurotic symptoms, jokes, mistakes, and the recalled contents of dreams. And
    more systematic evidence may be sought through more structured research methods such
    as surveys, quasi-experiments, experiments, and longitudinal studies of the psychosexual
    development of infants, youth, and adults. The following are some actual hypotheses
    derived from psychodynamic theory and its foundational propositions.
    Hypotheses
    1. Panic attacks are caused by a buildup or discharge of unfulfilled libidinal urges (e.g.,
    sex drive).
    2. Individuals suffering from panic attacks do not have a healthy sex life.
    3. Individuals with panic attacks who subsequently develop a healthy sex life, with
    regular orgasms, will experience a reduction in panic attacks.
    (I am not implying that the above hypotheses are anything but representative speculations to be found in this theory’s literature.) To test these hypotheses, one must operationally define the variables involved, and thus variables are an important aspect of any
    theoretical structure.
    Variables
    Panic attacks can be defined in various ways, but the operational definition found in the
    text-revised fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
    (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) is the most widely used approach to conceptualizing this phenomenon. Defining a healthy sex life is of course subject to widely varying
    interpretations, but such definitions would likely involve sexual activities that at least
    occasionally lead to orgasm. And the experience of an orgasm could be described in physiological and affective terms. With these operational definitions in mind, we could then
    try and test our hypotheses by gathering pertinent data, with observations and empirical
    evidence also being an important part of any theoretical structure.
    Observations or Empirical Evidence
    Any good theory for social work should be supported by a credible body of empirical evidence, information systematically gathered about the world using both reliable and valid
    methods of assessment and capable of being replicated by others. Omit any of these adjectives—systematic, reliable, valid, or replicable—and you have strayed from the domain of
    science. Evidence can take many forms, including but not limited to clinical observations,
    correlational studies, historical analysis, naturalistic studies, epidemiological studies, singlesubject studies, preexperiments, case control studies, quasi-experiments, time-series
    designs, randomized controlled trials, meta-analyses, and systematic reviews, among
    other types. Generally, the more good evidence bearing on a given theory, the better. The
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    475
    more this evidence consistently points to a particular conclusion (e.g., the theory is corroborated, or the theory is refuted), even better still.
    We could attempt to test Hypothesis 1 listed above using a variety of methods. One
    approach would be to obtain a sample of adult individuals who experience panic attacks
    and a demographically similar group who do not. We could ask them to retrospectively
    create a diary of sexual activities for, say, the past 2 weeks, specifically noting their orgasmic experiences. If the panic-prone individuals reported fewer orgasms than the nonpanickers, we could assert that the hypothesis was corroborated or supported and that the
    evidence provides some tentative evidence of one of Freud’s propositions. Or, if no differences were found, we would note that the hypothesis was not confirmed (or disconfirmed, if you prefer stronger language) and that a selected Freudian proposition thereby
    weakened (or falsified, if you prefer stronger language). But this is not the only way we
    could test Hypothesis 1. A perhaps stronger method would be to ask our sample of panickers and nonpanickers to prospectively keep a sex diary of orgasmic experiences and
    could then examine the results to see if the reports differed between our two groups. In
    science, prospectively collected data are usually accorded greater weight as credible evidence than is retrospectively obtained information. A third way to test Hypothesis 1
    would be to examine nonpanicking individuals who subsequently experienced prolonged
    periods of sexual abstinence (e.g., religious people who take vows of chastity, crewmembers on submarines), to see if the predicted development of panic attacks occurs.
    Hypothesis 3 might be tested by following a cohort of sexually abstinent panickers over
    time, some of whom later naturally develop sexual relationships and some of whom do
    not, and see if the frequency or intensity of panic attacks diminishes in the former group.
    Perhaps the reader could conceive of a more experimental or deliberately manipulative
    way to test Hypothesis 3?
    Over time, perhaps decades, heuristic hypotheses derived from theoretical propositions get tested in various ways, experimental and nonexperimental, and the validity or
    invalidity of the theory itself correspondingly waxes and wanes. Ideally, incorrect theories
    are eventually discarded, replaced by more fruitful, valid ones, recognizing, of course, that
    any conclusions regarding a theory’s truthfulness are best regarded as provisional and
    tentative, always subject to be revised as new or better data accumulate (Thyer, 1997). A
    theory may be revised in light of new data but should not be so elastic in its propositions
    that it is impossible to falsify. Such a theory lacks one of the essential features of a genuinely scientific accounting and will ultimately prove a nonproductive path.
    What Are the Characteristics of Good Theories?
    The theories used in social work range from the very weak to the scientifically strong.
    There is some consensus that the stronger scientific theories possess most if not all of the
    attributes listed in Table 26.2. But it must be admitted that relatively few theories in social
    work can be said to completely adhere to these rigorous benchmarks. Take some time to
    review these standards, derived from a variety of social work and non–social work sources
    (e.g., Chafetz, 1978; Polansky, 1975; Thyer, 2008a). A number of rating scales have
    appeared in the social work literature whereby one is supposedly enabled to quantify or
    rank the extent to which a given theory is said to possess selected attributes (reviewed in
    Daley et al., 2006). Some even allow you to come up with an overall score and thus hierarchically rank the theories used in social work. I do not see this as a particularly useful task
    and do not recommend the exercise to my readers. None of these scales have demonstrated
    476 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    sufficient acceptable levels of interrater reliability to make their findings trustworthy.
    Most, in fact, ignore the issue altogether, rendering any such ratings as liable to subjective
    bias. Each of the features listed in Table 26.2 can thus be said to be points for one to consider in appraising a given theory, recognizing that all will be found wanting to some
    extent. Other considerations may also be relevant, but the list in Table 26.2 can be seen as
    minimally essential qualities.
    T A B L E 26.2
    Some Features Characteristic of a “Good” Theory
    The Theor y—
    is determinist—A good theory of human behavior, like theories for all natural phenomena, seeks
    explanations for individual and social behavior in terms of environmental and biological factors, without
    recourse to metaphysical or supernatural explanations. Determinism does not assert that free will does
    not exist; rather, it focuses our attention on factors capable of scientific investigation and potentially
    amendable to prediction and control.
    is comprehensive—A good theory for social work practice should be as comprehensive as possible. A
    theory that is developed to account for an isolated clinical phenomenon (e.g., depression) is not as
    valuable as another theory that yields a plausible theory of depression, schizophrenia, personality
    disorders, anxiety disorders, and so on.
    is rational—A good theory must be rational. Given certain assumptions, subsequent deductions must not
    violate the rules of logic, generate tautological statements, or yield contradictory conclusions.
    is heuristic—A heuristic theory is one that generates predictive hypotheses capable of being tested and
    in general stimulates empirical research to examine these hypotheses. A theory that generates little
    research is seen as a less valuable one than a more productive approach.
    is parsimonious—A good theory is one that effectively explains a large group of phenomena using
    as few general principles as possible. Whenever one can choose between two theoretical
    explanations, parsimony suggests that the simpler of the available and adequate accounts is more
    likely to be true. Derived from the philosophical principle called Occam’s razor (from the 14thcentury philosopher William of Occam), parsimony can be expressed as “what can be explained by
    the assumption of fewer things is vainly explained by the assumption of more things” (see R.
    Epstein, 1984, p. 122).
    is operational—A good theory uses clear and concise definitions of terminology, concepts, and variables
    comprising that theory. Its language should lend itself to reliable and valid measurement.
    avoids reification—To reify is to attribute reality status to concepts and explanatory factors that as of yet
    have no real evidence that they truly exist.
    avoids circular reasoning—Circular reasoning occurs when one invokes as an explanation a variable
    whose only evidence for existence is that which it is said to cause. To say that a person runs away
    from dogs because he or she has a phobia of dogs, when the only existence for the phobia is the
    running away, is an example of this error. Saying that the reason why someone drinks too much is
    because he or she has alcoholism, when the only evidence for the alcoholism is excessive drinking,
    is another example. It is more likely to occur when metaphorical terms are reified and then used to
    explain things.
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    477
    is falsifiable—The hypotheses derived from a given theory must be capable of being tested and of being proved
    false. This can weaken a theory, and enough diverse instances of such falsification eventually cause the theory
    to be discarded as incorrect. If a hypothesis (e.g., a prediction derived from a theory) is tested and the results
    come out as predicted, we typically do not claim that the theory has been proven to be true. Rather, we say
    that the theory has survived a test and been supported, corroborated, or strengthened. Since the supported
    findings of any given theory (X) are subject to being overturned or subsumed by a better, more comprehensive
    theory (Y), social work theorists are usually cautious about making assertions that a given theory has been
    proven to be true.
    is practical—For a theory to be practical for social workers, it should have some direct applications to
    practice or policy. A theory lacking such practical applications is inherently less valuable for the human
    service professions than one that does provide such guidance.
    is empirical—A good theory is one that (eventually) is buttressed by solid empirical evidence. In general,
    a theory with more relevant empirical evidence is to be preferred over a theory with less levels of
    supportive evidence. A theory that does not lend itself to empirical testing at all, within the human
    services, is generally seen as having little value.
    has clarity—A good theory is clearly understandable by reasonably educated professionals. It is generally
    free of jargon, and those unique terms or neologisms legitimately required by the theory are well defined
    and operationalized.
    NOTE: These characteristics of a “good” theory are not listed in any priority order. All are important.
    Where Do Theories Come from?
    There is a wide array of sources of inspiration that can go into creating theoretical advances,
    sources that are available to virtually all social workers and thus enable any one of us—
    academic, practitioner, or student alike—to make potentially valuable theoretical contributions. There is a common view that significant theoretical advances are made primarily by
    singularly creative individuals blessed with a gift of genius that enables them to produce relatively complete conceptual frameworks almost fully formed, like the goddess Athena
    sprang forth from the forehead of Zeus. And those of us lacking this gift of perhaps divinely
    inspired creativity are fated to never be capable of crafting important theoretical advances.
    Those of us lacking a theoretical muse can take heart from learning about the actual process
    of theory development from well-known individuals responsible for genuine theoretical
    breakthroughs. In this regard, the French genius Louis Pasteur said a couple of pertinent
    statements, one being “Chance favors the prepared mind.” And the second, “Let me tell you
    the secret that has led me to my goal: My strength lies solely in my tenacity.” If not the
    French Pasteur, how about the Americans Thomas Edison—“Genius is one percent inspiration and ninety-nine percent perspiration”—and Margaret Mead—“I learned the value of
    hard work by working hard” and “The way to do fieldwork is never to come up for air until
    it is all over,” or the British Thomas Huxley—“Patience and tenacity are worth more than
    twice their weight of cleverness” (all quotes retrieved from www.brainyquote.com). Here
    then are two important secrets—learn as much as you can about your subject matter and
    then work very hard, perhaps for a long time. Without adequate preparation and effort, you
    are virtually guaranteed to never create a useful theory. With these qualities, you have a
    chance at least to do so. Randolph-Seng (2008) provides these observations:
    478 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    To theorize about something, you need to know a lot about it. The more knowledgeable you become about an area. . . , the more likely you will be able to identify
    gaps in existing theory and be inspired by your informed intuition an imagination
    to construct/extend a theory to bridge them. In turn, these bridges must be
    strengthened by constructive criticism and accumulation of evidence. (p. 39)
    OK, hard work and intellectual preparation are crucial. But what are some other
    sources of theoretical advancement? Perhaps the greatest source of information that leads
    to theoretical advances in the human services consists of clinical observations, the impressions gleaned, sometimes systematically but often not, by our experiences as practitioners
    interacting with our clients. We listen to their stories, their histories, their truths, and
    sometimes their lies, and we glean corroborative information from their family members,
    peers, colleagues, and coworkers. We observe one instance of something with one client,
    repeated instances of the same thing with the same clients, and/or repeated instances of
    the same thing across a number of clients. If, over time, patterns seem to emerge, we may
    develop one or more theoretical accounts of the how and why of the phenomena on question. As Newmark and Beels (1994) suggested,
    In psychotherapy, and in family therapy in particular, theories are rarely tested or
    even challenged by any quasi-experimental methodology. Instead they are constructed from observations of repetitive patterns in the therapist’s office. In this
    sense they are drawn from a type of natural history methodology, rather than from
    experimental science. (p. 13)
    A specific example can be drawn from Sigmund Freud’s practice with clients suffering
    from anxiety neurosis, work that led him to the following clinical observation:
    In the case of agoraphobia etc. we often find the recollection of an anxiety attack;
    and what the patient fears is the reoccurrence of such an attack under the special
    circumstances in which he believes he cannot escape. (Freud, 1962, p. 81)
    This clinical observation, coupled with other information about his anxious patients,
    led to an etiological theory that anxiety attacks were defenses against unacceptable sexual
    urges, such as to act promiscuously or to masturbate, leading to the avoidance of public
    places characteristic of agoraphobia. Apart from these and similar clinical observations,
    there were little empirical data to corroborate or refute Freud’s views for many decades.
    Aware of this gap, while employed as a clinical social worker at a specialty anxiety disorders program, I conducted a small-scale survey study of 28 members of an agoraphobia
    self-help group I was advising and found that every one did report having a history of
    panic attacks, consistent with Freud’s observations (Thyer & Himle, 1985). Moreover,
    when asked to provide an estimated date for the first panic attack and the emergence of
    their agoraphobia, there was a 9-year lag, with panics preceding agoraphobia. Fully 79%
    endorsed the statement that their panics caused their agoraphobia (as opposed to their
    agoraphobia caused their panics or that their panics and agoraphobia were unrelated), all
    consistent with Freud’s clinical impressions. Thus, in terms of descriptive features, Freud’s
    clinical observations have been supported by this simple survey and similar studies.
    However, his idea that panic attacks are caused by undesired sexual urges has not been
    supported, thus illustrating the problematic nature of clinical observations—sometimes
    they provide accurate leads, but sometimes they do not, and it can be difficult to sort out
    truthful clinical impressions from the false ones.
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    479
    Apart from clinical observations, one’s skilled observations of so-called normal
    persons, children, youth, and adults can provide rich grain for the millstones of theory.
    Jean Piaget, the Swiss developmental psychologist, derived much of his theories pertaining to cognitive development from observing his own children. While tremendously
    influential for many decades, Piaget’s theory of mind has not stood up well to empirical
    testing and is no longer widely considered to be a viable model for understanding cognitive development among children. Again, while clinical impressions and anecdotes alone
    are great starting places for theoretical advancements and conceptualizing problems and
    treatments, they are generally seen as inadequate methods to separate valid theories from
    erroneous ones.
    Prior existing theory is of course a productive inspiration for subsequent theoretical
    developments. As Isaac Newton is said to have stated, “If I have seen further it is by standing on the shoulders of giants.” Reinventing the wheel is never a good use of time, and if
    one is thoroughly conversant with prior theoretical formulations surrounding an issue,
    one’s contributions are more likely to represent an advance as opposed to an unknowing
    reiteration of conceptual discoveries previously made by others. However, not all epistemological frameworks within social work research take such a benign view of the effects
    of being immersed in prior theory. Many perspectives drawn from the field of qualitative
    inquiry take quite the opposite stance, suggesting that one’s familiarity with prior theory
    can bias or distort one’s observations (Shaw & Compton, 2003). It has been suggested by
    some qualitative methodologists that researchers should not deliberately immerse themselves in prior theory until they have completed their data collection, so as to avoid this
    problem. This perspective is, however, a minority one.
    Serendipity, or the role of chance, can also play a role in stimulating theoretical developments. The German chemist Kerkulé is said to have discovered the ring-like structure
    of the chemical benzene by having a daydream about snakes holding their tail in their
    mouths (an ancient mythological symbol). My own work as a clinician brought me into
    contact with clients who suffered from trichotillomania (TTM), or compulsive hair
    pulling. One evening, I was having drinks and dinner at the home of one of my doctoral
    students, Patrick Bordnick, who, serendipitously, had two cockatoos (large, parrot-like
    birds) as pets. As we consumed our adult beverages, we watched with amusement the
    antics of the birds walking about the room, preening themselves, climbing curtains, and
    so on. Pat began telling me about a behavioral problem called feather-picking disorder
    (FPD) that can occur in birds. We began laughing over the apparent similarities between
    trichotillomania in humans and feather-picking disorder in birds. What began as a bibulous conversation subsequently led to a legitimate investigation of the two literatures,
    TTM and FPD; the recruitment of an avian veterinarian coauthor; and an eventual conceptual linking, resulting in a serious theoretical paper published in a psychiatric journal
    suggesting that FPD has the potential to be an animal model of TTM in humans
    (Bordnick, Thyer, & Ritchie, 1994). Were it not for the chance of Pat and myself being
    together with the cockatoos and cocktails, this theoretical paper might never have been
    written. Serendipity—it is a wonderful thing.
    One very legitimate source of crafting theoretical research is to extrapolate, to take
    some existing theory and to apply it to a new field. Social work has a very long tradition
    of doing this, to the point of being criticized in some circles for having so little indigenous
    theory to point to, that is, disciplinary theory developed solely by social workers for use by
    social workers. Rather, we have tended to borrow existing theory from related disciplines
    such as psychiatry, psychology, and sociology and then present how this theory has etiological, assessment, or practice applications within social work. Psychoanalytic theory is
    of course the prototypical example of this type of theoretical borrowing, which ended up
    480 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    having tremendous influence within the field of social work (Eisler, 1942; Goldstein,
    2002); learning theory is another (Fischer & Gochros, 1975). More contemporary theoretical borrowings have come from chaos theory (Hudson, 2000), the quantum theory of
    physics (Weick, 1991), and innumerable others. One becomes acquainted, perhaps superficially, with a theoretical framework originating in some other discipline; becomes
    intrigued with its possible applications to social work’s etiological understanding of some
    psychosocial issue, assessment, or intervention; delves more deeply into the new theory;
    and, in due course, prepares a paper introducing this new theory to the social work literature. If the approach is seen to have merit, others may pick up the topic and elaborate on
    it, and slowly, perhaps, a sizable literature will emerge extrapolating this conceptual
    framework to our discipline, often over decades.
    Social worker Mark Mattaini (1991) engaged in this type of theoretical research when
    he applied some principles of operant psychology to the problem of crack cocaine abuse.
    Dr. Mattaini used operant principles, previously not applied to this practice problem, to
    help conceptualize how crack use is so highly addictive and to propose various methods,
    all based on operant principles (matching theory, analysis of antecedents and consequences surrounding crack use, discriminative stimuli, etc.), to prevent and treat crack
    use at the level of individual, agency, community, and policy initiatives.
    Similarly, social work faculty member Karola Dillenburger (2007) attempted to integrate operant theory with the field of practice known as victimology, the study of why
    certain people become victims of crime. By looking at factors such as an individual’s
    learning history, prevailing contingencies of reinforcement and punishment, establishing
    operations, the transgenerational transmission of trauma, behavioral economics, and cultural contingencies, Dillenburger fleshed out a beginning theoretical approach to this
    important emerging field of study.
    My own background in learning theory has led me to prepare a number of articles and
    chapters, attempting to apply (extrapolate) certain aspects of this approach to various
    problems within social work. One of the most interesting such projects I have engaged in
    was to apply social learning theory principles to try and understand the causes of racial
    prejudice (Arhin & Thyer, 2004). By examining the literatures on observational learning,
    operant learning, classical conditioning, and attitude formation, we attempted to develop
    a theoretical model of why and how racially prejudiced behavior and attitudes are
    acquired and maintained by individuals and to provide some leads on how racial prejudice may be reduced or eliminated.
    In another example, one from the animal learning theory literature, I knew of a phenomenon called punishment-induced aggression (PIA). PIA is observed when, for
    example, you have two animals (e.g., two rats or two pigeons) contained in a small cage,
    and one animal only is subjected to random, brief, but painful electric shocks. A common
    reaction is for the shocked animal to attack the innocent bystander animal contained in
    the cage with it. It seems as if the animal exposed to aversive stimuli “lashes out” at its
    partner, who of course has nothing to do with the first animal’s exposure to painful shock.
    I was also aware of the literature on the antecedents to child abuse, many of which are
    macro-scale variables (e.g., poverty), but one commonly reported micro-scale phenomenon is the frequent clinical observation, buttressed by larger scale systematic survey studies, that adult child abusers frequently lash out against children after the adult has been
    exposed to some aversive event. It may be something the child does (enuresis, encopresis,
    crying, irritating questioning, leaving a toy for the adult to trip over, etc.) or events completely unrelated to the child (an argument at work or with one’s spouse, receipt of bad
    news, pain, etc.), which adversely affect the adult. Here is how one authority on child
    abuse describes a frequent dynamic found in such families:
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    481
    Abuse incidents initiate with some behavior on the part of the child which the
    parent . . . perceives as aversive. (Kadushin & Martin, 1981, p. 253)
    Also,
    Aversive events are at the root of angry aggression. . . . Regardless of whether the
    aversively stimulated person is aware of these expressive responses and memories at
    the time, these initial reactions produce a tendency to attack the perceived source of
    the unpleasantness. (Berkowitz, 1983, p. 112)
    The apparently spontaneous eruption of some instances of child battering bears a
    striking similarity to PIA observed in animals. How one construes (theorizes) the etiology
    of child abuse has an important bearing on how one undertakes to treat or prevent it. If
    you see the abuser as having a character flaw or psychodynamically construed developmental injury, then proper treatment will consist of some sort of therapy for the abuser.
    But if abuse is seen as related to PIA, then effective treatment and prevention will be
    aimed at eliminating the aversive events the abuser is exposed to, such as by providing
    treatment for a child’s chronic enuresis or intervention to alleviate chronic job stressors,
    if these are ascertained to be events that have evoked abuse in the past. This theoretical
    linking of PIA and child abuse was not intended as a comprehensive approach to understanding but as an admittedly small conceptual contribution that may have treatment
    applications (Thyer, 1987a). In any event, it seems undeniable that learning theory variables are implicated in the problem of child abuse.
    In another example of extrapolation, my clinical work exposed me to large numbers of
    persons with panic attacks and agoraphobia who displayed escape-like behaviors that
    seemed very much akin to superstition. Such people may be unable to venture out of the
    home without holding on to some object that provides a sense of security (e.g., sunglasses, canes, umbrellas, large purses, small bottles of liquor, or charms). They may
    engage in elaborate breathing exercises, counting rituals, or soothing affirmations, or they
    may carry bottles of ice water for drinking or spray bottles containing cold water for a
    quick misting of the face. Now both panic attacks and acute anxiousness caused by venturing outside the home tend to subside, even if the client does nothing, providing he or
    she remains in the anxiety-evoking situation without trying to escape. If someone has a
    panic attack or becomes highly anxious and uses some sort of comforting ritual or activity, he or she may well feel better after a few minutes and not unreasonably attribute this
    diminished apprehension to what he or she did (e.g., an affirmation or breathing exercise)
    as opposed to the natural subsidence of fear. Over time, these people can become quite
    dependent on these superstitious-like behaviors to function, believing that they are
    needed to reduce anxiety. This can inhibit a full recovery.
    Again drawing from the experimental and theoretical literature on learning theory, I
    was aware of a phenomenon called superstitious behavior, demonstrated experimentally
    in animals by Skinner (1948) and also among children and adults. The basic preparation
    in the lab involves exposing an animal to random positive reinforcement (e.g., food pellets) as it meanders around a cage. Over time, the animals often (not always) come to display some repetitive or stereotyped behavior that is actually unrelated to the receipt of
    reinforcement but nevertheless is engaged in quite reliably. I speculated (Thyer, 1986) that
    the “comfort” behaviors observed clinically among anxious clients might be akin to the
    phenomenon of superstitious behavior seen in animals and adults (e.g., bowler’s gyrations after releasing the ball, a gambler’s rituals prior to the throw of the dice). A favorable
    outcome (e.g., diminished fear for the anxious person, food for the rat) is attributed to
    482 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    what one was doing just prior to being reinforced with food or the relief of anxiousness.
    In reality, however, there is no “real” relationship going on. This was an admittedly speculative paper, but the model does provide some implications for practice, and that is what
    theoretical papers are intended to do.
    Sharing your nascent ideas with colleagues, fellow professionals, friends, or family can
    be a good way to help clarify your thoughts and to gain feedback about the clarity of your
    ideas and any gaps that may have escaped your attention. This can be done via formal
    brainstorming sessions held specifically for this purpose or, less formally, say, over a cup
    of coffee.
    Consult the empirical literature on creativity. Interestingly, there is a small but intriguing body of literature on the creative process. I particularly recommend R. Epstein (1996a,
    1996b) and his essay titled “How to Get a Great Idea” in the 1996b volume.
    Expose yourself to creative people and ideas. It has been said that creativity is infectious.
    One of the best ways to catch the creativity bug is to attend conferences, preferably unfamiliar ones. If you have not been to the annual conference of the Society for Social Work
    and Research, consider attending this high-quality meeting (see www.sswr.org). Seek out
    interdisciplinary meetings or those sponsored by other disciplines related to your areas of
    social work, such as the annual meetings of groups like the American Psychological
    Association, the American Sociological Association, the American Public Health
    Association, the American Evaluation Association, and the Campbell Collaboration
    (especially recommended; see www.campbellcollaboration.org). Attend presentations by
    world-class researchers and theoreticians, chat them up during receptions and other
    social events, participate in special interest groups, and so on. Very frequently, I come
    away from major conferences with new book ideas, a new article I want to write, newly
    made professional friends with whom I subsequently collaborate, a commitment for a
    new scholarly project, and so forth.
    Doctoral students are a particularly valuable resource for theoretical inspiration. If you
    are an academic with the opportunity to serve on doctoral dissertation committees or even
    to serve as a student’s major professor, such experiences often prove immensely productive
    in terms of stimulating theoretical thinking on your part. You must remain intellectually
    agile and will almost be forced to keep abreast of new developments in important areas of
    policy, practice, and theory to productively participate in doctoral education. Offer to
    teach a new class, one different from the stale old standbys you’ve been doing for years and
    ideally one with lots of theoretical content. University faculty are often allowed to take
    courses for free, and these can be terrific ways to bring you into contact with new theoretical developments. If you are a social worker with access to a university, take an advanced
    class in psychology, sociology, economics, or political science or attend colloquia, grand
    rounds, and seminars in new areas. If you are a student, cultivate the leading intellectual
    lights of your program—take their classes, ask to do an independent study with him or her,
    submit a jointly authored conference paper or journal article, and in general take advantage of all the opportunities afforded by the modern-day university for creative growth
    above and beyond the required program of study for your degree.
    Above all, begin by writing. Theories usually begin in embryonic form and take time to
    fully develop. If you wait until you have a thoroughly fleshed out theory, you may wait a
    very long time indeed until you begin to write about your original ideas. Write out your
    beginning proposition or assumptions, explanatory variables, proposed relationships, and
    possibly some predictive hypotheses and how they could be tested. Perhaps make an outline. Save it, and return to it periodically, adding paragraphs as your thoughts develop. This
    writing may not (indeed will likely not) reflect a linear progression. No matter. Perhaps
    keep a notebook to jot down your theoretical ideas or at least a dedicated documents file
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    483
    on your computer. When ready, share and discuss with others, as noted above. Use feedback to revise, tweak, and discard. Keep in mind that the best is the enemy of the good. If
    your standard is perfection, you will be immobilized by impractically high standards.
    Think of the flaws in existing grand, middle-range, and low-level theories and take comfort in knowing that if these can be floating around with all their problems and incompleteness, there is room in the world of scholarly publishing for your own contribution.
    While a bit florid, the advice of Pannapacker (2009) is pretty good:
    If you do not get an insight down on paper, and possibly develop it while your
    excitement lasts, then you are squandering the rarest and most unpredictable of
    your human capabilities, the very moments when one seems touched by the hand of
    God. (Pannapacker, 2009, p. B5)
    Theoretical Exegesis
    The term exegesis refers to critical commentary upon a text, and it is most often used
    when the commentary is provided on holy scriptures. I suspect that the largest proportion
    of theoretical writing to be found in the social work literature comprises this type of contribution, writing and commenting on one or more specific theories that are being used
    in social work, with the purpose of familiarizing the reader with that theory’s fundamental principles and actual or potential applications to social work policy and practice.
    Social workers typically come into contact with theoretical exegesis via undergraduate
    courses in psychology, sociology, and so forth and via authored textbooks used in our
    human behavior in the social environment (HBSE) courses. This type of writing can be
    helpful when a large amount of material must be covered or a smaller amount taught
    about a large number of theoretical perspectives. Of course, the latter information is often
    superficial at best, misleading at worst. Another avenue of communicating information
    about a large number of theories is to expose the students to edited books containing one
    or more chapters about a given theory, each authored by a presumptive expert in that
    theory. Some examples of comprehensive textbooks of this type include Roberts and Nee
    (1970), Dorfman (1988), Turner (1996), Brandell (1997), and Thyer (2008a). These allow
    somewhat more exposure to a given theory than the typical authored HBSE textbook, but
    even here chapters devoted to a particular approach may be only 20 to 30 pages long. A
    third method is an authored social work text that is devoted to a single theoretical perspective. This can be the most comprehensive approach to conveying theoretical information and is particularly good when the author is a legitimate, noted scholar with
    expertise in that theory. Some examples here include Strean (1978) and Gambrill (1987).
    Such textbooks are not common in our discipline because we have, as a discipline, comparatively few social workers who possess a reputation as serious original scholars and
    genuine experts within particular theoretical fields.
    Another drawback of the widespread practice of theoretical exegesis is that students
    may only develop a familiarity with a given theory via second- or thirdhand sources, not
    primary ones. Thus, their views are derived from someone else’s interpretations of a given
    theory, rather than what the originators of the theory themselves actually had to say.
    Sometimes this is helpful if the original writings are particularly dense or obtuse or not
    translated into English. Having a knowledgeable social work scholar who has made it his
    or her business to become thoroughly familiar with a given body of theoretical content
    and to transmute this large literature into a summary form more readily digestible by the
    484 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    B.S.W. or M.S.W. student can be a very helpful undertaking indeed. But this is only
    helpful if the translating (literally or figuratively) is faithfully and accurately undertaken.
    I myself have taken great offense at the misrepresentations to be found in the social work
    literature of my own preferred orientation, social learning theory, and have devoted a fair
    amount of time in documenting some of the more common errors and in providing corrective information (Myers & Thyer, 1994; Thyer, 1991, 2005). I can only imagine the
    indignation of my psychodynamic colleagues at the similar mishmash of psychodynamic
    theory, which is often found in social work writings.
    I recall participating in one doctoral student’s oral preliminary examination, wherein
    as a part of his writings he had prepared a critique of the philosophical position called
    logical positivism. I asked him if he could tell me who Rudolph Carnap (one of the
    founders of logical positivism) was. He could not. He had not heard of him. I then asked
    him if he could name any original source materials written by any of the founders of logical positivism that he read in preparation for writing his critique of logical positivism. He
    had read none, being content to study instead only the flimsy social work literature critical of this approach while never really coming into contact with what logical positivism
    really is! This is neither sound scholarship nor adequate conceptual preparation for social
    workers seeking a solid grounding in a given theoretical framework. If you want to learn
    about psychoanalytic theory, then for heaven’s sake read what Freud has to say, not (at
    least initially), what Smith says Jones had to say about what Freud wrote. If you want to
    claim expertise in learning theory, then read Skinner’s original articles and books, not
    commentaries upon commentaries upon Skinner. Once having developed this foundation of primary source materials, then go read contemporary commentaries and reviews,
    not claiming real familiarity with a given theory until you have achieved a good grasp of
    its foundational documents.
    Theoretical Criticism as Theoretical Research
    There seems to be a sort of Hegelian process operating within the social work theoretical
    literature in that shortly after a novel conceptual approach is introduced to our field (thesis?), articles appear criticizing this new approach (antithesis?). Sometimes the critiques
    take the form of contending that the presumed new theory has been inaccurately
    described or represented by the pioneering author. Other times, the response is to contend that, actually, the new theoretical borrowing is simply not applicable to the social
    work processes and phenomenon it was originally said to relate to. More rarely, the criticism invokes empirical data supposedly indicating the serious lack of evidence, if not evidence in complete juxtaposition to, the proposed new applications to social work. The
    new approach may be beaten down by the critiques, to reside unmourned and rarely cited
    within the pages of textbooks and journals. Or the new theory’s proponents may rally
    around and defend their approach, perhaps with vigor and perhaps overcoming the presumptuous critics (synthesis?). It is hoped that this is done in the dignified manner characteristic of scientific discourse, but this is not always the case. Recall that the purpose of
    theory is to bring us closer to nature’s truths. If a newly proposed or extrapolated theory
    (call it Theory X) is seen by some as taking us further away from nature’s truths, then
    some individuals may be prompted to criticize Theory X because of its perceived own
    shortcomings (a pure critique of X alone) or because X conflicts with the tenets of Theory
    Y or could perhaps be subsumed under Theory Y (a critique of X and a simultaneous promotion of Y). This too is legitimate theoretical critique.
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    485
    When I began my M.S.W. studies in the mid-1970s, the textbook we used in my human
    behavior theory class was called Human Behavior in the Social Environment: A Social Systems
    Approach (Anderson & Carter, 1974). This volume was the nemesis for a whole generation
    of students who, like myself, found its extrapolations of the science of general systems
    theory, ecology, and cybernetics (themselves very legitimate fields) to social work theory to
    be exceedingly hard to grasp and apply. I hated it. I felt I was stupid for being unable to
    ascertain its value to working with clients, thought the jargon dehumanizing, and suspected
    it was simply a Potemkin-like exercise to beef up social work’s pretenses at being scientifically based. However, I was always secretly bothered by the possibility that everyone else “got
    it,” and I did not. This was my burden of shame, and I was reluctant to reveal it to others.
    Imagine my delight when, two decades later, one of the smartest and most respected theoretical scholars in our field (I mean this sincerely), Jerome Wakefield (1996a, 1996b), wrote
    two brilliant and scathingly critical essays titled “Does Social Work Need the Eco-systems
    Perspective? Part 1: Is the Perspective Clinically Useful?” and “Does Social Work Need the
    Eco-systems Perspective? Part 2: Does the Perspective Save Social Work From Incoherence?”
    I read with interest and a sense of self-validation Wakefield’s conclusions that
    the perspective’s lack of explanatory power . . . is its undoing . . . the perspective
    cannot contribute to assessment, integration, or bias correction . . . a careful reading of ecosystems writings . . . readily reveals the clinical vacuity of the perspective . . . the continued widespread use of the ecosystems perspective could have dire
    consequences for the social work profession. (Wakefield, 1996a, pp. 27, 28)
    The perspective had drained social work of its person-centered orientation and
    commonsense vocabulary, obscured social work’s value-laden purpose, and made it
    seem that social work is an intellectually flaccid enterprise that depends on untested
    perspectives rather than supported theories. (Wakefield, 1996b, p. 208)
    There was some gentle pushback (Gitterman, 1996) but nothing that Wakefield could
    not handle well (Wakefield, 1996c). I highly commend this series of articles as illustrative
    of how one can make theoretical contributions to the field via the process of legitimately
    criticizing existing approaches. In lieu of the ecosystems perspective, Wakefield could not
    resist promoting his own preferred alternative unifying framework for social work practice, an approach known as distributive justice; thus, he was critiquing Theory X as well as
    proposing Theory Y as a preferred alternative. Thanks to Wakefield’s example, I was
    emboldened enough to engage in some catharsis of my own in a later critique of systems
    theory (Gallant & Thyer, 1999).
    Each of the major theories residing within social work has been subject to theoretical
    criticism. Here are a few representative examples:
    • Solution-focused therapy has been criticized by the feminists (Dermer, Hemesath,
    & Russell, 1998).
    • Feminism has been criticized by Lay and Daley (2007).
    • Afrocentric theory has been criticized by Pellebon (2007).
    • Psychodynamic theory has been criticized by Deal (2007).
    • Ecological theory has been criticized by Rotabi (2007).
    • Behaviorists have been criticized by the cognitivists (I. Epstein, 1975; Mahoney, 1989).
    • Cognitivists have been criticized by the behaviorists (Thyer, 1992).
    • Postmodernism (Brigham, 1977) has been criticized by Meinert (1998), Atherton
    and Bolland (2002), and Noble (2004).
    486 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    The list goes on and on, with each such contribution being authored by someone
    convinced that he or she is doing the profession a service by exposing the fallacies of
    Theory X and providing greater light upon the path in his or her promotion of Theory Y.
    And to be sure, this sometimes does happen, which is a very good thing indeed and truly
    represents an advance in our theoretical understanding. Other times, to be sure, the effort
    is little more than shoving knowledge around with little in the way of theoretical progress.
    Nevertheless, theoretical criticism remains a solid way of contributing to social work’s
    conceptual foundations.
    Another Example of Theoretical Research
    Below is a description of another research project I was engaged in that led to a couple of
    original theoretical speculations, speculations subsequently corroborated by independent
    investigators through their own data-based studies.
    Question—How Does Exposure Therapy for Phobias Work?
    In the early part of my career as a clinical social worker, I was employed in a specialty clinic
    that focused on providing services to clients with a primary anxiety disorder. Our interdisciplinary Anxiety Disorders Program was housed in a research-oriented Department of
    Psychiatry at the University of Michigan School of Medicine, and clinical and research
    roles were filled by psychiatrists, social workers, psychologists, nurses, and so on. By the
    1970s, it was pretty well established that treatments involving exposing clients to fearevoking situations under therapist-guided, carefully controlled, graduated conditions of sufficient duration were usually very effective in alleviating anxiety symptomatology associated
    with conditions such as specific and social phobias, panic disorder, agoraphobia, obsessivecompulsive disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder (Thyer, 1983, 1987b), a conclusion
    that remains upheld today (see Barlow, Allen, & Basden, 2007). What was not at all clear
    was how, exactly, did exposing fearful clients for prolonged periods of time to frightening
    events result in symptom relief? In other words, what were the mechanisms of action of
    exposure therapy? There were a number of candidates, such as the psychoanalytic theory
    of abreaction, various learning theories (habituation, desensitization, reciprocal inhibition, etc.), the so-called exposure effect written about by social psychologists, some emerging cognitive explanations, and various biologically based accounts.
    The University of Michigan at the time was a hub for research into the newly discovered compounds called endogenous opiates (EOPs), substances produced by the human
    brain under conditions of pain or great stress, and it had been speculated that EOPs may
    reduce fear and attenuate memories of fearful events. I had been involved in some prior
    research examining how the body responds to phobic anxiety in terms of endocrine and
    cardiovascular reactions (Nesse et al., 1985), and I used my familiarity with the EOP and
    endocrine release literature to speculate that phobic anxiety may also result in EOP stimulation. A physician colleague and I designed a simple A-B-A single-subject study with a
    voluntary client who had a severe phobia of snakes. The physician inserted a small needle
    into the client’s arm while she rested comfortably in a chair for about 60 minutes, during
    which time two blood samples were taken. Then I brought into the room, with the client’s
    informed consent, a large caged snake. I conducted exposure therapy with her for 30 minutes, attempting to keep her at the maximum level of anxiety she could tolerate. During
    this time, two more blood samples were taken. When the snake was removed, she again
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    487
    rested in the chair for 60 minutes, and two final blood samples were taken. We found that
    relative to the first baseline or A phase, during exposure therapy, her subjective anxiety
    became very high (self-rated at 90 points out of 100 maximum), her heart rate
    approached 150 beats per minute, and her levels of plasma beta-endorphin (a major variant of endogenous opiates produced by humans) also spiked. We concluded our report of
    this empirical finding (basic empirical research?) by speculating that “one possible mechanism for the therapeutic efficacy of prolonged exposure to anxiety-evoking stimuli is
    endorphenergic in nature” (Thyer & Mathews, 1986, p. 240). We then ventured in a small
    way into the realm of theoretical research by the following risky speculation:
    One test of this hypothesis would be to conduct a control group study of simple
    phobics receiving a standardized period of exposure therapy, half of whom would
    receive infusions of a narcotic antagonist during treatment while the other half
    would receive active placebo infusions. If this infusion of a narcotic antagonist partially blocked the therapeutic response to exposure, EOP mechanisms would be
    implicated as a biological substrate for the efficacy of such exposure. Prior to conducting such an experiment, replication of the present study with a group design is
    certainly indicated. (Thyer & Matthews, 1986, p. 240)
    Narcotic antagonists are sometimes used in the treatment of opiate abusers as they
    block the effects of drugs such as morphine or heroin. If clients who were pretreated
    with such compounds experienced reduced benefits from exposure therapy compared to
    clients not so pretreated, this could be seen as a robust test of the EOP hypothesis as an
    underlying biological mechanism at least partially related to the mechanism of action of
    exposure therapy.
    Subsequent empirical research has corroborated both of the hypotheses we put forth.
    Phobic anxiety does result in EOP activation (Carr, 1996; Egan, Carr, Hunt, & Adamson,
    1988), and pre-treating clients with an opiate blocker prior to exposure therapy does
    inhibit treatment response (Merluzzi, Taylor, Boltwood, & Gotestam, 1991). In the words
    of Kozak et al. (2007), “Results provide support for the endogenous opioid system as a
    potential underlying biological mechanism associated with behavioral changes during in
    vivo exposure” (p. 142). Now the issue is not settled, of course, but the above chronology
    extending over 20 years does illustrate how theoretical speculations can come to guide
    subsequent empirical research, which does, in turn, lead to further theory-based hypotheses. In this example, the original paper was empirical, but we concluded with a bit of theorizing, and it was this small proportion of the paper that constituted our theoretical
    contribution. This speculation, originating in a small-scale, indeed a single-subject, study
    (which is about as small as you can go), led to further empirical testing and corroboration
    by others, with the new data in turn feeding back upon theory.
    Summary
    Theoretical research is a rarely undertaken form of scholarship, but an adequate theoretical foundation for understanding the causes of psychosocial problems and the mechanisms of action of social work interventions will ultimately be an essential feature of a
    mature social work profession. We are a long way from this, and such empirically supported theoretical accounts will likely be developed using middle-range and lower range
    theories prior to the emergence of any comprehensive theoretical foundation covering the
    488 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    large and diverse scope of social work practice. It is essential that we continue to borrow
    the best applicable conceptual frameworks from related disciplines and also develop a
    stronger indigenous body of theory emerging from those talented social workers most in
    contact with the world of practice. There are a variety of ways in which we can contribute
    to the body of social work theory, including exegesis (bringing relevant theory to the
    attention of the field), extrapolation (commenting on how heretofore unfamiliar theoretical accounts may be applied to selected social work phenomena), and theoretical criticism. We remain most in need of new original conceptual explanations that possess the
    attributes of a good scientific theory; that lend themselves to testing, falsification, or corroboration; and whose results can be fed back into the existing body of theory in a recursive manner that results in an improved theory closer to nature’s truth. We need always to
    keep in mind the epitaph found on the tomb of Karl Marx, one of the most influential
    social theorists of the 19th and 20th centuries, whose ideas continue to influence contemporary political thought—“The philosophers have only interpreted the world in various
    ways—the point however is to change it.” The meaning and usefulness of social work
    theory reside in its consequences in action, as well as in fidelity to reality. A theory that
    does not ultimately produce helpful methods of alleviating human distress is a waste of
    time to the field. We have many such useless theories, and some are actively harmful
    (Thyer, 1994a, 1994b, 2008b).
    References
    American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th
    ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
    Anderson, R. E., & Carter, I. (1974). Human behavior in the social environment: A social systems
    approach. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
    Arhin, A., & Thyer, B. A. (2004). The causes of racial prejudice: A behavior-analytic perspective. In
    J. L Chin (Ed.), The psychology of prejudice and discrimination: Vol. I. Racism in America (pp. 1–
    19). Westport, CT: Praeger.
    Atherton, C. R., & Bolland, K. A. (2002). Post-modernism—A dangerous illusion for social work.
    International Social Work, 45, 421–433.
    Barker, R. (Ed.). (2003). The social work dictionary (4th ed.). Washington, DC: NASW Press.
    Barlow, D. H., Allen, L. B., & Basden, S. L. (2007). Psychological treatments for panic disorders,
    phobias, and generalized anxiety disorder. In P. E. Nathan & J. Gorman (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work (3rd ed., pp. 351–394). New York: Oxford University Press.
    Berkowitz, L. (1983). The experience of anger as a parallel process in the display of impulsive, “angry”
    aggression. In R. G. Geen & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds.), Aggression: Theoretical and empirical
    reviews: Vol. I. Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 103–133). New York: Academic Press.
    Bordnick, P. S., Thyer, B. A., & Ritchie, B. (1994). Feather-picking disorder and trichotillomania: An
    avian model of human psychopathology. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
    Psychiatry, 25, 189–196.
    Brandell, J. R. (Ed.). (1997). Theory and practice in clinical social work. New York: Free Press.
    Brigham, T. M. (1977). Liberation in social work education: Applications from Freire. Journal of
    Education for Social Work, 13(3), 5–11.
    Carr, J. E. (1996). Neuroendocrine and behavioral interaction in exposure treatment of phobia
    avoidance. Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 1–15.
    Causey, R. L. (1977). Unity of science. Boston: Reidel.
    Chafetz, J. S. (1978). Primer on the construction and testing of theories in sociology. Itasca, IL: F. E.
    Peacock.
    Daley, J. G., Peters, J., Taylor, R., Hanson, V., & Hills, D. (2006). Theory discussion in social work
    journals: A preliminary study. Advances in Social Work, 7(1), 1–19.
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    489
    Deal, K. H. (2007). Psychodynamic theory. Advances in Social Work, 8, 184–195.
    Dermer, S. B., Hemesath, C. W., & Russell, C. S. (1998). A feminist critique of solution-focused
    therapy. American Journal of Family Therapy, 26, 239–250.
    Dillenburger, K. (2007). A behavior analytic perspective on victimology. International Journal of
    Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 3, 433–447.
    Dorfman, R. A. (Ed.). (1988). Paradigms of clinical social work. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
    Egan, K. J., Carr, J. E., Hunt, D. D., & Adamson, R. (1988). Endogenous opiate system and systematic desensitization. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 287–291.
    Eisler, E. R. (1942). Technical problems of social work in relation to clinical psychoanalysis.
    American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 12, 1991–2002.
    Epstein, I. (1975). The politics of behavior therapy: The new cool-out casework? In H. Jones (Ed.),
    Towards a new social work (pp. 138–150). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
    Epstein, R. (1984). The principle of parsimony and some applications in psychology. Journal of
    Mind and Behavior, 5, 119–130.
    Epstein, R. (1996a). Cognition, creativity, and behavior: Selected essays. Westport, CT: Praeger.
    Epstein, R. (1996b). Creativity games for trainers: A handbook of group activities for jumpstarting
    workplace activity. New York: McGraw-Hill.
    Fischer, J., & Gochros, H. (1975). Planned behavior change: Behavior modification in social work.
    New York: Free Press.
    Freud, S. (1962). Obsessions and compulsions. In S. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 3, p. 81). London: Hogarth.
    Gallant, J. P., & Thyer, B. A. (1999). Usefulness of general systems theory in social work practice. In
    R. R. Greene (Ed.), Human behavior theory and social work practice (2nd ed., pp. 250–257).
    New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
    Gambrill, E. (1987). Behavior modification. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
    Gambrill, E. (1994). What’s in a name? Task-centered, empirical and behavioral practice. Social
    Service Review, 68, 578–599.
    Gentle-Genitty, C. S., Gregory, V., Pfahler, C., Thomas, M., Lewis, L., Campbell, K., et al. (2007). A critical
    review of theory in social work journals: A replication study. Advances in Social Work, 8(1), 62–80.
    Gitterman, A. (1996). Ecological perspective: Response to Professor Jerry Wakefield. Social Service
    Review, 70, 472–476.
    Goldstein, E. G. (2002). Psychoanalysis and social work: Historical perspectives. Psychoanalytic
    Social Work, 9(2), 33–40.
    Gray, M., & Webb, S. A. (2009). Social work theories and methods. London: Sage.
    Greenwood, E. (1957). Social work research: A decade of reappraisal. Social Service Review, 31, 311–320.
    Grinnell, R. M., & Unrau, Y. A. (2008). Social work research and evaluation: Foundations of evidencebased practice (8th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
    Hudson, C. G. (2000). At the edge of chaos: A new paradigm for social work? Journal of Social Work
    Education, 36, 215–230.
    Johnsson, E., & Svensson, K. (2005). Theory in social work: Some reflections on understanding and
    explaining interventions. European Journal of Social Work, 8, 419–433.
    Kadushin, A., & Martin, J. (1981). Child abuse: An interactional event. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
    University Press.
    Kozak, A. T., Spates, C. R., McChargue, D. E., Bailey, K. C., Schneider, K. L., & Liepman, M. R.
    (2007). Naltrexone renders one-session exposure therapy less effective: A controlled pilot
    study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 142–152.
    Lay, K., & Daley, J. D. (2007). A critique of feminist theory. Advances in Social Work, 8, 49–61.
    Lewis, H. (1971, January). Practice, science and professional education: Developing a curriculum, responsive to new knowledge and values. Paper presented at the Symposium on the Effectiveness of Social
    Work Intervention: Implications for Curriculum Change, Fordham University, New York.
    Loeb, M. (1960). The backdrop for social research: Theory-making and model-building. In L. S.
    Kogan (Ed.), Social science theory and social work research (pp. 3–15). New York: National
    Association of Social Workers.
    Mahoney, M. J. (1989). Scientific psychology and radical behaviorism: Important distinctions
    based on scientism and objectivism. American Psychologist, 44, 1372–1377.
    490 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    Martin, J., & Sugarman, J. (2009). Middle-ground theorizing, realism, and objectivity in psychology. Theory & Psychology, 19, 115–122.
    Mattaini, M. A. (1991). Choosing weapons in the war on ‘Crack’: An operant analysis. Research on
    Social Work Practice, 1, 188–213.
    Meinert, R. G. (1998). Consequences for professional social work under conditions of postmodernity. Social Thought, 18(3), 41–54.
    Merluzzi, T. V., Taylor, C. B., Boltwood, M., & Gotestam, K. G. (1991). Opioid antagonist impedes
    exposure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 425–430.
    Mischel, W. (2009). Becoming a cumulative science. APS Observer, 22(1), 3, 18.
    Myers, L. L., & Thyer, B. A. (1994). Behavioural therapy: Popular misconceptions. Scandinavian
    Journal of Behaviour Therapy, 23, 97–107.
    Nesse, R. M., Curtis, G. C., Thyer, B. A., McCann, D. S., Huber-Smith, M., & Knopf, R. F. (1985).
    Endocrine and cardiovascular responses during phobic anxiety. Psychosomatic Medicine, 47,
    320–332.
    Newmark, M., & Beels, C. (1994). The misuse and use of science in family therapy. Family Process,
    33, 3–17.
    Noble, C. (2004). Postmodern thinking: Where is it taking social work? Journal of Social Work, 4,
    289–304.
    Pannapacker, W. A. (2009, February 20). How to procrastinate like Leonardo da Vinci. The
    Chronicle Review, pp. B4–B5.
    Pellebon, D. A. (2007). An analysis of Afrocentricity as theory for social work practice. Advances in
    Social Work, 8, 169–183.
    Polansky, N. A. (1975). Theory construction and the scientific method. In N. A. Polansky (Ed.),
    Social work research (Rev. ed., pp. 18–37). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    Queralt, M. (1996). The social environment and human behavior: A diversity perspective. Boston:
    Allyn & Bacon.
    Randolph-Seng, B. (2008). On becoming a theorist in psychology. Association for Psychological
    Science Bulletin, 21(8), 39–40.
    Reid, W. R. (1992). Task strategies: An empirical approach to clinical social work. New York:
    Columbia University Press.
    Roberts, R., & Nee, R. (Eds.). (1970). Theories of social casework. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    Rotabi, K. S. (2007). Ecological theory origin from natural to social science or vice versa: A brief
    conceptual history for social work. Advances in Social Work, 8, 113–129.
    Rubin, A., & Babbie, E. R. (2008). Research methods for social work (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thompson.
    Shaw, I., & Compton, A. (2003). Theory, like mist on spectacles, obscures vision. Evaluation, 9, 192–204.
    Skinner, B. F. (1948). “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.
    Stein, H. D. (1960). The concept of the social environment in social work practice. Smith College
    Studies in Social Work, 30(3), 187–210.
    Strean, H. S. (1978). Clinical social work: Theory and practice. New York: Free Press.
    Thyer, B. A. (1983). Treating anxiety disorders with exposure therapy. Social Casework, 64, 77–82.
    Thyer, B. A. (1986). Agoraphobia: A superstitious conditioning perspective. Psychological Reports,
    58, 95–100.
    Thyer, B. A. (1987a). Punishment-induced aggression: A possible mechanism of child abuse?
    Psychological Reports, 60, 129–130.
    Thyer, B. A. (1987b). Treating anxiety disorders. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
    Thyer, B. A. (1991). Behavioral social work: It is not what you think. Arete, 16(2), 1–9.
    Thyer, B. A. (1992). The term ‘cognitive-behavior therapy’ is redundant. The Behavior Therapist,
    15(5), 12, 128.
    Thyer, B. A. (1994a). Are theories for practice necessary? No! Journal of Social Work Education, 30,
    147–151.
    Thyer, B. A. (1994b). Is psychoanalytic therapy relevant for public mental health programs? No. In
    S. A. Kirk & S. D. Einbinder (Eds.), Controversial issues in mental health (pp. 123–128). Boston:
    Allyn & Bacon.
    Thyer, B. A. (1997). Is it possible to know when theories of human behavior are obsolete? Yes. In
    M. Bloom & W. Klein (Eds.), Controversial issues in human behavior (pp. 64–71, 79–80).
    Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
    C HAPTER 26  T HEORETIC AL R ESEARCH
    491
    Thyer, B. A. (2001a). Research on social work practice does not benefit from blurry theory. Journal
    of Social Work Education, 37, 51–66.
    Thyer, B. A. (2001b). The role of theory in research on social work practice. Journal of Social Work
    Education, 37, 9–25.
    Thyer, B. A. (2005). The misfortunes of behavioral social work: Misprized, misread, and misconstrued. In S. A. Kirk (Ed.), Mental health in the social environment: Critical perspectives (pp.
    330–343). New York: Columbia University.
    Thyer, B. A. (Ed.). (2008a). Comprehensive handbook of social work and social welfare: Vol. 2. Human
    behavior and the social environment. New York: Oxford University Press.
    Thyer, B. A. (2008b). The potentially harmful effects of theory in social work. In B. A. Thyer (Ed.),
    Comprehensive handbook of social work and social welfare: Vol. 2. Human behavior and the social
    environment (pp. 519–541). New York: Oxford University Press.
    Thyer, B. A. (2008c). The quest for evidence-based practice? We are all positivists! Research on Social
    Work Practice, 18, 339–345.
    Thyer, B. A., & Himle, J. (1985). Temporal relationship between panic attack onset and phobic
    avoidance in agoraphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 607–608.
    Thyer, B. A., & Matthews, J. (1986). The effects of phobic anxiety on plasma beta-endorphin: A single-subject experiment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 237–241.
    Thyer, B. A., & Myers, L. L. (1997). Behavioral and cognitive theories. In J. Brandell (Ed.), Theory
    and practice in clinical social work (pp. 18–37). New York: Free Press.
    Tolson, E., Reid, W. J., & Garvin, C. (1994). Generalist practice: A task centered approach. New York:
    Columbia University Press.
    Tripodi, T., Fellin, P., & Meyer, H. J. (1969). The assessment of social research. Itasca, IL: F. E. Peacock.
    Turner, F. (1979). Theory in social work practice. In F. Turner (Ed.), Social work treatment (pp. 1–
    12). New York: Free Press.
    Turner, F. (Ed.). (1996). Social work treatment. New York: Free Press.
    Wakefield, J. C. (1996a). Does social work need the eco-systems perspective? Part 1. Is the perspective clinically useful? Social Service Review, 70, 1–32.
    Wakefield, J. C. (1996b). Does social work need the eco-systems perspective? Part 2. Does the perspective save social work from incoherence? Social Service Review, 70, 183–213.
    Wakefield, J. C. (1996c). Does social work need the ecological perspective? Reply to Alex Gitterman.
    Social Service Review, 70, 476–481.
    Weick, A. (1991). The place of science in social work. Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare, 18(4),
    13–34.
    Westen, D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed
    psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 333–371.
    USEFUL WEB SITES
    The National Association of Social Workers’ Research
    http://www.socialworkers.org/research/default.asp
    The Program Theory and Theory-Driven Evaluation Thematic
    Interest Group of the American Evaluation Association
    http://www.mechanisms.org/
    The group is devoted to using program theory to improve the design, implementation, and evaluation
    of social and educational programs.
    Journals
    Although there are no journals explicitly devoted to social work theory, there are a large number of theoretical journals to be found in the behavioral and social sciences, such as the following:
    492 PART V  C ONCEPTUAL R ESEARCH
    Theory & Psychology
    http://tap.sagepub.com/
    Theory, Culture and Society
    http://tcs.sagepub.com/
    Political Theory
    http://ptx.sagepub.com/
    European Journal of Social Theory
    http://est.sagepub.com/
    Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy: An International Journal of Theory and
    Practice
    http://www.wiley.com/WileyCDA/WileyTitle/productCd-CPP.html
    Psychotherapy Theory, Research, Practice and Training
    http://www.apa.org/journals/pst/
    DISCUSSION QUESTIONS
    1. Review recent social work journals and try to locate an article devoted to explaining a given theory
    or advancing our theoretical understanding of some psychosocial phenomena. Bring this article to
    class and describe what it is about to your classmates.
    2. Chose a theory of interest to you and evaluate it in terms of its adherence to the features deemed
    essential to a scientifically “good” theory, as outlined in Table 26.2. In what ways does it meet these
    standards? In what ways is it inadequate?
    3. Ask your instructor if he or she has a preferred theoretical orientation, and if so, ask your instructor
    to describe how she or he came to adopt this particular conceptual framework and how, in his or her
    opinion, it stands up to the list of criteria characteristic of a good theory, as found in Table 26.2.

    Still stressed from student homework?
    Get quality assistance from academic writers!

    Order your essay today and save 25% with the discount code LAVENDER