You will research and read one academic article for each annotated bibliography, preferably on a topic that will go beyond the content you have learned so far and that delves deeper into an area of interest that will benefit you.
- Research Criteria: An article from an academic journal found through EBSCOhost databases at EU.
- Writing Criteria: In 300 words (Time Roman, 12-font, double-spaced, 5-6 sentences), summarize your article and indicate how it will be a useful reference for Part IIB.
Teacher Influences 1
Chen, J., Jiang, H., Justice, L. M., Lin, T.-J., Purtell, K. M., Ansari, A. (2020). Influences of
teacher-child relationships and classroom social management on child-perceived peer social
experiences during early school years. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2746.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586991
Influences of Teacher-Child Relationships and Classroom Social Management on Child-
Perceived Peer Social Experiences during Early School Years
Jing Chen
Hui Jiang
Laura M. Justice
Tzu-Jung Lin
Kelly M. Purtell
Arya Ansari
The Ohio State University
Acknowledgement
The research reported here was supported by the Institute for Education Sciences, through Grant
R305N160024 awarded to The Ohio State University (Justice). The opinions expressed are those
of the authors and do not represent views of the Institute or National Center for Education
Research. We would like to thank the research team, staff, and families without whom this
research would not have been possible.
Teacher Influences 2
Abstract
Interactions with teachers and peers are critical for children’s social, behavioral, and academic
development in the classroom context. However, these two types of interpersonal interactions in
the classroom are usually pursued via separate lines of inquiries. The current study bridges these
two areas of research to examine the way in which teachers influence child-perceived peer social
support and peer victimization for 2,678 children within 183 classrooms in preschool through
grade three. Two levels of teacher influence are considered, namely teacher-child closeness and
conflict relationships at the child-level, and teacher management of interpersonal interactions at
the classroom-level. Results of multilevel regression models showed that teacher-child closeness
was associated with the growth of child-perceived peer social support from fall to spring,
whereas teacher-child conflict and teachers’ behavior management practices were associated
with the change in child-perceived peer victimization across the academic year. These
associations were unique and above and beyond the influence of children’s actual peer social
interactions, including reciprocal friendships and the collective classroom reputation of peer
victimization. Collectively, findings highlight the multi-faceted teacher roles in shaping
children’s perceptions of their peer social experiences during the earliest years of schooling.
Keywords: Child-perception of peer social experiences; peer social support; peer victimization;
teacher-child relationships; classroom social management
Teacher Influences 3
Influences of Teacher-Child Relationships and Classroom Social Management on Child-
Perceived Peer Social Experiences during Early School Years
Children’s interactions with their teachers and peers are both salient features of the
classroom environment and figure prominently in theories concerning children’s development
and learning (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006). Studies find that positive interactions with
teachers and with peers and the way in which teachers manage interpersonal interactions in the
classroom influence children’s concurrent and long-term social, emotional, and academic
development (e.g., Hosan & Hoglund, 2017; Jerome et al., 2009; Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996;
Ladd et al., 2017). However, much of the extant literature has considered the influence of
teachers and peers separately (Hughes & Im, 2016; Wang et al., 2016). Consequently, we know
little about the roles of teachers in optimizing children’s perceptions of their peer social
experiences and whether teacher influences are above and beyond children’s actual peer social
interactions. This an important gap in knowledge because researchers have argued that children’s
perceptions of their peer social experiences might be more predictive of their social and
psychological well-being and school success than their actual peer interactions (Betts et al.,
2013; Önder et al., 2019; Troop-Gordon et al., 2019). Thus, the current study examines multiple
levels of teacher influences, including teacher-child relationships (i.e., closeness and conflict) at
the child-level and teachers’ classroom management of interpersonal interactions at the
classroom-level, on two aspects of peer social experiences from children’s perspective: peer
social support and peer victimization.
Significance of Child-Perceived Peer Social Experiences
Peer social support and peer victimization are two important aspects of children’s
classroom experiences. Peer social support refers to supportive behaviors from peers that can
Teacher Influences 4
enhance children’s functioning and resilience to difficulties (Bakalım & Taşdelen-Karçkay,
2016). Bakalım and Taşdelen-Karçkay argued that peer social support provides children with
emotional comfort that protects children against anxiety and stress, helps them cope with
difficulties via guidance and feedback. Indeed, peer social support is associated with a range of
positive outcomes, including children’s motivation, attention, academic attitudes, and
achievement (Bursal, 2017; Coolahan et al., 2000). Thus, peer social support is considered as a
primary indicator of school adaptiveness and academic success from preschool through
elementary school and above (Blandon et al., 2010; Coolahan et al. , 2000).
Peer victimization, on the other hand, has been linked with school maladjustment, which
refers to physical and emotional harms children receive from peers, such as being hit and teased.
Studies find that peer victimization is a precursor of loneliness and school avoidance (Buhs &
Ladd, 2001; Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996) and is associated with low self-esteem, depression,
external behavioral problems, and academic failure (Alsaker, 1993; Blandon et al., 2010; Ladd et
al., 2017; Olweus, 1992). Researchers report that children who experience peer victimization
tend to be less engaged classroom activities, which, in turn, is associated with their emotional
adjustment difficulties and limits their access to opportunities and resources that are essential for
social and academic development (Blandon et al., 2010; Buhs & Ladd, 2001).
Although children’s perceptions of their peer social experiences are related to their actual
peer social interactions (Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996), only a few studies have conceptually
differentiated children’s perceived peer experiences from their actual peer experiences. This
differentiation is important because some researchers suggest that perceptions of being supported
by peers reflect children’s competency in peer interactions, which is associated with their
learning behaviors and school success (Blandon et al., 2010; Coolahan et al., 2000). Specifically,
Teacher Influences 5
in the literature of peer isolation, the distinction between objective isolation and perceived
isolation has been established, with the former representing the actual quantity of peer
interactions and the latter capturing loneliness or the feeling of being isolated by peers (Danese
et al., 2009; Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009).
Differentiating perceived from actual peer social experience is also meaningful because
children’s perceptions might be more strongly associated with their social and emotional well-
being. On the one hand, children’s perceptions of their peer social experiences can shape their
self-perceptions or self-worth, which can then influence children’s social behaviors (Ogelman et
al., 2019) and their levels of being liked by peers (Önder et al., 2019). Önder et al. explained that
self-perception reflects one’s own competence and personality, which is established when
children perceive their strengths and weaknesses when interacting with others and that children
with low self-perception are likely to be passive and timid in peer interactions, which would
contribute to their being less liked by peers. On the other hand, Troop-Gordon et al. (2019)
discussed that support and victimization experiences in peer groups build children’s beliefs about
peers, which, according to social information processing theories, would shape their behavioral
and emotional responses to future interpersonal events. Some suggest that perceived isolation
tends to result in more severe and enduring consequences than objective isolation, because the
perceptions of being isolated can alter individuals’ social reasoning and information processing
(Danese et al., 2009; Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009). Specifically, Cacioppo and Hawkley
explained that the perception of being isolated by peers may trigger children’s confirmatory and
memorial bias and can lead to their negative interpretations of peers’ social moves, which in turn
may contribute to children’s misbehaviors and emotional maladaptiveness. Hence, although
perceived and actual peer social experiences are rarely distinguished in the broader sense of peer
Teacher Influences 6
social experience, it stands to reason that perceived peer social support and perceived peer
victimization would shape children’s understandings about themselves and about others.
Therefore, there is a need to examine factors that may influence children’s perceptions of their
peer social experiences.
Teacher Influences on Peer Social Experiences
Besides peers, teachers represent another key dimension of classroom ecology (Hamre &
Pianta, 2001; Jerome et al., 2009). As noted earlier, however, interactions with teachers and
interactions with peers tend to be discussed separately (Hughes & Im, 2016; Wang et al., 2016),
except for only a few studies as elaborated below; such work has suggested that teachers’
relationships with individual children and their classroom social management can shape
children’s peer social
experiences in the classroom.
For individual children, their interactions with teachers matter to their social experiences
with peers. This is because teacher-child interactions can be observed by all classmates, which
helps classmates draw inferences about children’s attributes and likeability and form a classroom
consensus about children’s reputations (Hughes & Im, 2016). Further, teacher-child closeness is
grounded in positive interactions, such as warm and open communications, between a teacher
and a child (Birch & Ladd, 1997), which forms a secure base for children to feel being cared and
connected to the classroom environment. Teacher-child closeness is associated with children’s
engagement in classroom activities and their social competences and peer acceptance (e.g., Birch
& Ladd, 1997; Gest & Rodkin, 2011; Hall-Lande et al., 2007; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004).
Children with close relationships with teachers may also receive greater support from teachers,
which contributes to their social and academic development (Hamre & Pianta, 2001). On the
contrary, teacher-child conflicts contribute to peer disliking as well as school avoidance,
Teacher Influences 7
externalizing behaviors, and decreased prosocial behaviors and cooperation (Hamre & Pianta,
2001; Hughes & Im, 2016).
At the classroom-level, teachers’ classroom management of interpersonal interactions
(i.e., classroom social management) serves to shape children’s peer social experience. Classroom
social management is a challenge and critical task for teachers, which requires them to be aware
of children’s social needs and to afford developmental opportunities for children to positively
interact with peers from diverse backgrounds (Farmer, Hamm, Dawes, Barko-Alva, & Cross,
2019). A commonly used tool to capture classroom social management is the Classroom
Assessment Scoring System (CLASS; Downer et al., 2012; Hamre & Pianta, 2007; Pianta et al.,
2008), which features three domains of classroom management based on social and instructional
interpersonal interactions (i.e., emotional support, classroom organization, and instructional
support). These three domains are further categorized into nine dimensions. The current study
includes four dimensions that mainly focus on the social aspect of interactional interactions,
naming positive climate, which refers to interactions between teachers and children and among
children that feature enthusiasm, enjoyment, and respect; negative climate, which refers to
classroom interpersonal interactions that involve anger, aggression, or harshness; teacher
sensitivity, which represent the extent to which teachers provide comfort, reassurance, and
encouragement based on individual children’s needs; and behavior management, which refers to
teachers’ effectiveness in preventing and redirecting children’s misbehaviors. Warm and
sensitive interactions with teachers and well-managed classrooms promote classroom
inclusiveness and facilitate social connections among children, through which children develop
social and emotional competences, reduce problematic behaviors, and become less vulnerable to
peer victimizations (Cappella & Neal, 2012; Downer et al., 2012; Hamre & Pianta, 2001).
Teacher Influences 8
Although teachers can influence children’s peer social experiences via multiple avenues
as reviewed above, few studies have taken into account different levels of teacher influences
simultaneously. Farmer et al. (2019) discussed that teachers are not only members in the
classroom society interacting directly with individual children, but, at the same time, they also
are leaders who act as an authority and a facilitator to manage classroom dynamics and to ensure
children following the rules. Hence, the current study aims to capture teachers’ multi-faceted
roles to have a more comprehensive understanding of teacher influence on children’s peer social
experiences in the classroom.
The Current Study
The current study focuses on children from preschool through grade three; during these
grades, positive peer experiences provide essential support to children’s development and
learning, whereas peer victimization occurs relatively more often than that in the later grades
(Ladd et al., 2017; Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996). Thus, there is a need to investigate teacher roles
in managing classroom social dynamics during children’s primary years of schooling.
Although there has been some research examining certain teacher influence on children’s
peer social experiences, it is not clear whether teacher influences operate above and beyond the
influence of children’s actual peer social interactions. For the purpose of this study, children’s
actual peer interactions were operationalized as the number of reciprocal friendships and their
classroom reputation of peer victimization. Friendship is considered as the most important source
of peer support, which provides children with a context for skill acquisition and development and
helps children to validate their shared beliefs and identifies (Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003;
Ladd et al., 1996). Further, compared to unilateral friendships (i.e., one child identifies the other
as a friend but not vice versa), reciprocal friendships (i.e., children mutually identify each other
Teacher Influences 9
as friends) tend to have higher quality, are more stable, and, therefore provide greater peer
support (e.g., Quinn & Hennessy, 2010). Classroom reputation of peer victimization reflects the
consensus among all classmates about the extent of harassment one experiences from peers.
Hughes and Im (2016) discussed that children’s disliking of a child tends to go beyond dyadic
antipathy and would be contributed greater by group-based reputation based on shared
observations. Both reciprocal friendship and classroom reputation of peer victimization
triangulate the perceptions from both children and peers, which, therefore, would be less biased
by individuals’ opinions.
In all, the current study aims to examine multiple levels of teacher influence on child-
perceived peer social support and peer victimization in the spring of the academic year when
controlling for those in the fall. Teacher influences include teachers’ closeness and conflict with
individual children and their classroom social management at the classroom-level as represented
by observations of positive climate, negative climate, teacher sensitivity, and behavior
management. A sub-aim is to determine whether the above teacher influences on children-
perceived social experiences are unique and operate beyond the influence of their actual peer
interactions manifested as the number of reciprocal friendships and classroom reputation of peer
victimization.
Methods
Participants
This study is part of a large federally funded project focused on advancing understanding
of early childhood learning experiences from preschool (pre-kindergarten) to third grade. The
study sample consisted of two cohorts of participants, recruited from two large school districts in
Teacher Influences 10
a Midwestern state. Recruitment procedures were carried out in accordance with protocols to
protect human subjects as approved by the institutional review board (IRB) of the university.
Before the school year started, informational sessions were held in schools located within
district borders to recruit teachers. All children in classrooms taught by participating teachers
were eligible to enroll, and consent packets were sent home via backpack mail. Most participants
were recruited in the fall, although additional preschool classrooms were added in winter and
spring to meet recruitment goals. Consented teachers were asked to complete questionnaires
about their classrooms, their children, their teaching practices, and their own background
information. Consented children were administered direct assessments in fall and spring of the
school year.
The sample included 43 schools, 183 classrooms, and 2,678 consented children. As
summarized in Table 1, 50% of the participating children were girls, 66% were White, and 13%
were Hispanic/Latino(a). Twelve percent of the children came from households that primarily
spoke a language other than English and 10% of children had identified disabilities. Annual
family income was distributed bimodally with 27% of the participating families falling in the
lowest income bracket ($30,000 or lower) and 31% in the highest income bracket ($120,001 or
higher). Forty-five percent of the children’s mothers completed four-year college education or
higher. At the classroom level, an average classroom had 22 children (range = 12 ~ 29). Teachers
were mostly female (97%), White (96%), and non-Hispanic (99%). On average, they were 38
years old with 13 years of teaching experience. Ninety-four percent of the teachers had a
bachelor’s degree or higher, and 82% had a teaching certificate.
Measures
Teacher Influences 11
To address the aims of the current study, we included measures of child-perceived peer
social experiences, teacher-child relationships, classroom social management, and actual peer
social interactions. Children’s family background and demographic information were collected
from caregiver and teacher questionnaires at the beginning of the school year.
Child-perceived peer social experiences. In fall and spring of the school year, one-on-
one child interviews were conducted by trained research staff in quiet areas of the school
hallway, and responses were recorded using a tablet in accordance with the approved study
protocols. Based on previous studies of peer relationship and children’s school adjustment
(Asher et al., 1984; Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996; Ladd, 1990; Waters et al., 2012), the research
team developed measures of perceived peer social support comprising a total of 11 items (e.g.,
“How often would kids in your class help you if you are hurt?” and “How often would kids in
your class tell you you’re good at things?”) and perceived peer victimization consisting of four
items (e.g., “Does anyone in your class ever hit you?” and “Does anyone in your class ever say
mean things to you?”). All items used a three-point frequency scale (0 = Never, 1 = Sometimes, 2
= A lot), and the internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) ranged from .75 to .78 across scales and
time points. The responses from items on the same scale were averaged to create composite
scores for each child. In the analysis, spring scores were used as outcomes, and fall scores were
included as covariates.
Teacher-child relationships. In the fall, teachers reported on their closeness and conflict
with each child using the Student-Teacher Relationship Scale (Pianta, 1991). The closeness
subscale included seven items (e.g., “I share an affectionate, warm relationship with this child”
and “If upset, this child will seek comfort from me”) and the conflict subscale contained eight
items (e.g., “This child and I always seem to be struggling with each other” and “Dealing with
Teacher Influences 12
this child drains my energy”). All items used a five-point Likert-type scale (0 = Definitely does
not apply, 4 = Definitely applies) and the scales demonstrated strong internal consistency (alphas
ranged from .88 to .94). For analysis, the mean score of each subscale of the teacher-child
relationship was calculated for each child.
Classroom social management. Teacher’s classroom social management was captured
in the winter with the Classroom Assessment Scoring System (CLASS, Pianta et al., 2008). As
noted earlier, although the original CLASS includes nine dimensions, the current study focuses
on four dimensions mainly from the social domain, including 1) positive climate, which reflects
the warmth, respect, and enjoyment communicated by verbal and non-verbal interactions, 2)
negative climate, which assesses the overall level of expressed negativity among teachers and
children in the classroom, 3) teacher sensitivity, which refers to the teacher’s awareness and
responsiveness to the various needs of individual children and the entire class, and 4) behavior
management, which encompasses the teacher’s use of clear behavioral expectations and effective
methods to prevent and redirect misbehavior. In each classroom, trained and reliable research
staff conducted two 30-minute observation cycles, where observers live-coded the teacher’s
practice or behavior as it contributed to the overall classroom environment on scales of 1 to 7 (1
= minimally characteristic, 7 = highly characteristic). Composite scores for each dimension
were created by averaging across the two cycles. To ensure reliability, research staff completed
extensive training sessions before entering the field, and ongoing quality checks were conducted
via biweekly drift meetings. In addition, 20% of all in-field observations were double-coded, and
inter-rater agreement (i.e., two coders scored within one point of difference on the same
dimension) ranged from .90 to .92.
Teacher Influences 13
Peer social interactions. Peer social interactions including reciprocal friendships and
classroom reputation of peer victimization were collected in the spring based on a peer
nomination approach (Parkhurst & Asher, 1992), which has been found valid for children as
young as preschoolers (Chen et al., 2020; Daniel et al., 2016). We asked children to identify
classmates “who are your best friends” and “who gets picked on or teased?” Preschoolers were
presented with a photo roster of all children in their classrooms to facilitate the nomination,
while older children were provided a list of names of their classmates. For each child, we
counted the number of reciprocal friendships when the child and classmates mutually nominated
each other as best friends; classroom reputation of peer victimization was represented by the
frequency at which the child was nominated by classmates as someone who gets picked on or
teased. Children’s raw scores were standardized by dividing classroom size minus one, the
maximum possible value, to allow the indices to be compared across classrooms.
Analytical approach
We employed multilevel regression models to investigate the effects of teacher influence
on children’s perception of peer social experiences, given that children (level-1) were nested
within classrooms (level-2). Two outcomes were examined, namely the child-perceived peer
social support and child-perceived peer victimization in the spring. For each outcome, we first
ran unconditional multilevel models where child outcomes were clustered by classrooms, to
determine the percentage of observed variance attributable to classroom differences. Second, we
fitted conditional multilevel models (Model 1), examining the association between teacher-child
relationships and teacher classroom management and child-perceived peer social experiences,
controlling for the pretest scores (i.e., child-perceived peer social experiences in the fall). Other
controlled variables included child gender, disability status reported by teachers in spring, child
Teacher Influences 14
race reported by caregivers (dichotomized into White vs. non-White), grade level, and school
district. Finally, we included actual peer social interactions (i.e., reciprocal friendship and
classroom reputation of peer victimization) as covariates to test whether teacher influences
contribute to children’s perceptions above and beyond their actual peer social interactions
(Model 2). All models were fit in R with the lmer package (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker,
2015) with maximum likelihood estimation. Missing data were list-wise deleted. The proportion
of missing for each variable is reported in Table 1.
Results
As shown in Table 2, children generally perceived that they had some peer social support,
both in fall and in spring (mean = 1.32 and 1.35) with 75-79% reporting scores between 1
(Sometimes) and 2 (A lot). The mean of child-perceived victimization was 0.44 and 0.53 in the
fall and spring, respectively, with 32-34% of children reporting never experiencing peer
victimization. A little over one-half of children (55% in fall and 51% in spring), however,
perceived experiencing some victimization, with scores greater than 0 (Never) but less than 1
(Sometimes). In terms of teacher-child relationships, teachers reported moderate to high levels of
closeness (M = 3.13 out of 4) and low levels of conflict (M = 0.63 out of 4). Additionally, the
classrooms were rated as having moderate quality in terms of teacher sensitivity (M = 4.65 out of
7), behavior management (M = 5.42 out of 7), and positive climate (M = 5.52 out of 7), and were
scored very high in the area of negative climate (suggesting the absence of negativity; M = 6.92
out of 7). Finally, in terms of actual peer social interactions, children had reciprocal friendships
with 8% of their classmates (range = 0%~38%) and were nominated as “being picked on or
teased” by 4% of their classmates (range = 0%~80%).
Teacher Influences 15
Pairwise correlations are presented in Table 3. There was a moderate correlation between
child-perceived peer experiences in the fall and the spring (.41~.55). Child-perceived peer
victimization was negatively correlated with teachers’ behavior management scores (-.25~-.20),
and child-perceived peer victimization in the spring was also negatively correlated with teachers’
ability to promote a positive climate (-.16). In addition, teacher-child closeness and conflict were
negatively correlated (-.27), and the four CLASS indices were positively correlated (.17~.70).
Teacher influences on child-perceived peer social experiences
The primary aim of the current study was to examine the associations between teacher-
child relationships and teachers’ classroom social management and two aspects of child-
perceived peer social experiences in the spring: peer social support and peer victimization. The
unconditional model (Model 0, output not presented) showed that for perceived peer social
support, 3% of the variance (< .01) was attributable to differences between classrooms, and 97%
(.15) was due to individual differences. For perceived peer victimization, 14% of the variance
(.04) was accountable by classroom-level differences, while 86% of the variation (.25) was
between children.
Next, our focal teacher predictors of interest were included in Model 1 (Table 4). Results
showed that, after controlling for fall responses on child-perceived peer social experiences and
other covariates, teacher-child closeness significantly predicted child-perceived peer social
support (b = 0.04, p < .01) and teacher-child conflict predicted child-perceived peer victimization
(b = 0.10, p < .001). Specifically, with one additional unit increase in teacher-child closeness (on
a scale of 0 to 4), child-perceived peer social support was expected to increase by 0.04 units (on
a scale of 0 to 2). With one unit increase in teacher-child conflict, child-perceived peer
victimization was expected to increase by 0.10 units. At the classroom level, teachers’ behavior
Teacher Influences 16
management was negatively associated with child-perceived peer victimization (b = -0.07, p
< .05). A unit increase in behavior management (on a scale of 1 to 7) was associated with 0.07
unit of decrease in child-perceived peer victimization. Collectively, Model 1 accounted for
approximately 20% of the variance for both of the outcome variables at the child level, and over
70% of the variance at the classroom level for child-perceived peer victimization. Almost no
extra classroom-level variance for child-perceived peer social support was accounted for by the
above variables, which might be because there was originally little classroom-level variance
(4%) in total as suggested by the unconditional model.
Finally, to determine whether the associations reported above were unique, we included
children’s actual peer social interactions in Model 2 (Table 5), which were operationalized as
reciprocal friendships and classroom reputation of peer victimization. Results showed that even
though reciprocal friendship was a strong predictor of child-perceived peer social support (b =
0.74, p < .001) and classroom reputation of peer victimization was predictive of self-perceived
peer victimization (b = 0.94, p < .001), the above-reported association associations were stable
and remained significant.
Discussions
The current study examined the interplay among teachers, children, and peers as actors in
the classroom social ecology during early school years. Specifically, we focused on the
influences of teacher-child closeness and conflict and teacher’s classroom social management on
child-perceived peer social support and peer victimization. The current study expands on the
existing literature by, first, simultaneously taking into account teachers’ roles as classroom
members who form closeness and conflict with individual children and as leaders who shape
classroom social dynamics, and, second, by further highlighting the critical roles of teachers in
Teacher Influences 17
shaping children’s perceptions of their peer social experiences, after controlling for children’s
actual peer social interactions. The major findings are discussed below.
First, it is evidenced that teacher influence at the individual-level and that at the
classroom-level are unique, and that each contributes to child-perceived peer social experiences.
In terms of the relationships between teacher and individual children, our findings showed that
teacher-reported closeness and conflict with children in the fall contributed to peer social support
and peer victimization perceived by children in the spring respectively, controlling for the fall
scores. This finding indicates that children with close relationships with teachers tend to feel
more socially supported by peers and that children who have conflicts with teachers tend to
experience increased perceived peer victimization over the academic year. These findings are in
line with the literature that teacher-child interactions broadcast children’s attributes and likability
to classmates who observe the interactions (Hughes & Im, 2016), which foster a classroom
consensus regarding children’s reputations and therefore influence classmates’ interactions with
the children. It is also likely that positive teacher-child relationships can promote children’s
cooperative engagement in classroom activities and improves their social competence, while
with negative teacher-child relationships, children may avoid school and demonstrate more
externalizing behavior problems and less prosocial behaviors during interpersonal interactions
(Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Hughes & Im, 2016).
Second, regarding teachers’ classroom social management, our findings showed that
better behavior management in the fall was associated with less peer victimization as perceived
by children in the spring controlling for the fall scores. This finding suggests that in classrooms
where misbehaviors are better managed and redirected, child-perceived peer victimization
decreases over time. This finding is aligned with literature showing that well-managed
Teacher Influences 18
classrooms are associated with greater social and academic development and with reduction of
behavior problems (Emmer & Stough, 2001; Downer et al., 2012). Kochenderfer-Ladd and
Pelletier (2008) further discussed that, when teachers do not consider bullying as a normative
behavior in the classroom, they would be more likely to intervene towards negative peer social
interactions rather than expecting the victims to handle the incidences on their own, which has
been found associated with lower levels of peer victimization in the classroom.
However, it is surprising that the other classroom social management indicators (i.e.,
teacher sensitivity, positive climate, and negative climate) were not found to be positively
associated with child-perceived peer social experiences in the current study. It might be that the
influence of teacher sensitivity and classroom climate on children’s classroom social experiences
might be more indirect than behavioral management and could take a longer time to alter
children’s peer social experiences. Another possibility from the measurement perspective is that,
as reported in the result section, there was minimal variance at the classroom-level in the
unconditional model when predicting children-perceived peer social support, which left little
room for the classroom-level teacher influences to show predictive effect. Future research may
apply a more refined tool to assess these aspects of the classroom ecology.
A third major finding is that teacher influences on children’s perceptions of their social
experiences operate in a manner that is unique and beyond children’s actual peer social
interactions. Specifically, for children who are similar in the number of reciprocal friendships
and in the collective classroom reputation of peer victimization, those who have close
relationships with their teachers perceived having greater peer social support, whereas those who
had conflicted relationships with their teachers perceived greater peer victimization. Also, those
in classrooms with better behavior management perceived less peer victimization.
Teacher Influences 19
Children’s perceptions of their peer social experiences emerge based on their social
interactions, which then may reflect their self-evaluation of social competence as well as beliefs
about peers (Blandon et al., 2010; Coolahan et al., 2000). Our results indicate that as a member
and an authoritative figure in the classroom, teachers play a critical role in shaping children’s
beliefs about their own strengths and weakness in social interactions and about the classroom
social environment, which operates uniquely beyond the influence of children’s actual peer
social interactions. It is possible that, independent from actual interactions with peers, positive
relationships with teachers and well-managed classrooms can enhance children’s sense of
connectiveness with classmates, which improves their social competence in engaging in peer
social interactions (Hughes & Im, 2016), and can promote the classroom inclusiveness; in turn,
this may reduce problematic social behaviors and help children become less vulnerable to peer
victimizations (Cappella & Neal, 2012). However, the current study does not draw causal
inferences. Future study is needed to examine the mechanism and dynamic relations among
teachers, peer social interactions, and children’s perceptions of their peer social experiences.
Despite these contributions to the literature, there are a few limitations in the current
study. First, teacher-child relationships were assessed at a single time point. However, these
relationships may vary across the academic year, as suggested by Hughes and Im (2016) who
showed that the average one-year stability of teacher-child closeness and conflict were .38
and .57 in elementary classrooms. Similarly, although children’s perceptions of peer social
experiences were assessed in the fall and spring and classroom social management was observed
multiple times in the winter, it is necessary for future studies to account for the change
throughout an academic year in terms of children’s perceived classroom social experiences and
teachers’ classroom social management. Second, at the classroom-level, teachers can shape
Teacher Influences 20
classroom interpersonal interactions through many other ways besides classroom social
management, such as seating arrangements, grouping strategies, types of activities, and
responsibilities afforded to children (Farmer et al., 2019). While the current study has taken into
account multiple levels of teacher influences, future research may take a more systematic and
comprehensive view when examining teacher influences on classroom social dynamics. Third,
when representing children’s actual peer social interactions, although the current study tried to
select the most representative indicators (e.g., reciprocal friendships and classroom reputation of
peer victimization), other aspects of peer social interactions can contribute to perceived peer
social support, such as peer acceptance, peer rejection, and peer isolation. Future research may
consider applying a latent-variable approach to account for different aspects of peer social
interactions when representing children’s actual social experiences. Fourth, children’s
perceptions provide a unique perspective of their peer social experiences. However, their
perceptions can be biased, and so can teacher reports of their relationships with children. Future
studies may consider using more objective measures to capture peer social experience and
teacher-child relationships. Finally, caution is warranted when generalizing findings from the
current study. Although the study sample represented a wide range of families from diverse
backgrounds, families were drawn from two school districts in a single Midwestern state in the
U.S. Additionally, teachers who were willing to participate in this study and to be observed by
researchers might have demonstrated relatively higher classroom social management skills
considering the majority of them had a bachelor’s degree or higher. Accordingly, replication
with different samples, measures, and methods is an important future direction.
In all, the current study demonstrated that teachers can influence children’s perceptions
of their peer social experiences simultaneously through their closeness and conflict with
Teacher Influences 21
individual children and through their classroom social management. Additionally, such teacher
influences on children’s perceptions are unique from children’s actual peer social interactions.
Findings underscore the need for teachers to develop close relationships with individual children
and to eliminate conflict with them. As Hughes and Im (2016) suggested, although it is
understandable that teachers might report conflict with children who have problem behaviors,
teachers are encouraged to provide support to these children so as to optimize their classroom
experiences. Beyond interactions with individual children, as the leaders in the classrooms,
managing and redirecting misbehaviors can improve the quality of interpersonal interactions and
reduce negative peer social experiences perceived by children. In sum, the current study
highlights the multi-faceted roles of teachers in shaping children’s classroom experiences and the
classroom social ecology during the earliest years of schooling.
Teacher Influences 22
References
Asher, S. R., Hymel, S., & Renshaw, P. D. (1984). Loneliness in children. Child Development,
1456–1464.
Bakalım, O., & Taşdelen-Karçkay, A. (2016). Friendship quality and psychological well-being:
the mediating role of perceived social support. International Online Journal of Educational
Sciences, 8(4), 1–9.
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models
using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Betts, L. R., Rotenberg, K. J., & Trueman, M. (2013). Young children’s interpersonal trust
consistency as a predictor of future school adjustment. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 34(6), 310–318.
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher-child relationship and children’s early school
adjustment. Journal of School Psychology, 35(1), 61–79.
Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Grimm, K. J., Keane, S. P., & O’Brien, M. (2010). Testing a
developmental cascade model of emotional and social competence and early peer
acceptance. Development and Psychopathology, 22(4), 737–748.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In
R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child development: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human
development (6th ed., pp. 793–828). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Buhs, E. S., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as antecedent of young children’s school
adjustment: An examination of mediating processes. Developmental Psychology, 37(4),
550–560.
Bursal, M. (2017). Academic achievement and perceived peer support among Turkish students:
Teacher Influences 23
Gender and preschool education impact. International Electronic Journal of Elementary
Education, 9(3), 599–612.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 447–454.
Cappella, E., & Neal, J. W. (2012). A classmate at your side: Teacher practices, peer
victimization, and network connections in urban schools. School Mental Health, 4(2), 81–
94.
Chen, J., Lin, T.-J., Jiang, H., Justice, L., Purtell, K., & Logan, J. (2020). Triple Alignment:
Congruency of Perceived Preschool Classroom Social Networks among Teachers, Children,
and Researchers. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1341. https://doi.org/doi:
10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01341
Coolahan, K., Fantuzzo, J., Mendez, J. L., & Mcdermott, P. (2000). Preschool peer interactions
and readiness to learn: Relationships between classroom peer play and learning behaviors
and conduct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 458–465.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.458
Danese, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., Polanczyk, G., Pariante, C. M., … Caspi,
A. (2009). Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease:
depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Archives of Pediatrics
& Adolescent Medicine, 163(12), 1135–1143.
Daniel, J. R., Santos, A. J., Antunes, M., Fernandes, M., & Vaughn, B. E. (2016). Co-evolution
of friendships and antipathies: A longitudinal study of preschool peer groups. Frontiers in
Psychology, 7, 1509. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01509
Downer, J. T., López, M. L., Grimm, K. J., Hamagami, A., Pianta, R. C., & Howes, C. (2012).
Teacher Influences 24
Observations of teacher–child interactions in classrooms serving Latinos and dual language
learners: Applicability of the Classroom Assessment Scoring System in diverse settings.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(1), 21–32.
Emmer, E. T., & Stough, L. M. (2001). Classroom management: A critical part of educational
psychology, with implications for teacher education. Educational Psychologist, 36(2), 103–
112.
Farmer, T. W., Hamm, J. V, Dawes, M., Barko-Alva, K., & Cross, J. R. (2019). Promoting
inclusive communities in diverse classrooms: Teacher attunement and social dynamics
management. Educational Psychologist, 54(4), 286–305.
Gest, S. D., & Rodkin, P. C. (2011). Teaching practices and elementary classroom peer
ecologies. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 288–296.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2011.02.004
Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2003). Childhood peer relationships: Social
acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. Journal of School Psychology, 41(4), 235–284.
Graham, S., Bellmore, A., & Juvonen, J. (2003). Peer victimization in middle school: When self-
and peer views diverge. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 19(2), 117–137.
Hall-Lande, J. A., Eisenberg, M. E., Christenson, S. L., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2007). Social
isolation, psychological health, and protective factors in adolescence. Adolescence, 42(166),
265–287.
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory of
children’s school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development, 72(2), 625–638.
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2007). Learning opportunities in preschool and early elementary
classrooms. In R. Pianta, M. Cox, & K. Snow (Eds.), School readiness & the transition to
Teacher Influences 25
kindergarten in the era of accountability (pp. 49–84). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.
Hosan, N. E., & Hoglund, W. (2017). Do Teacher–Child Relationship and Friendship Quality
Matter for Children’s School Engagement and Academic Skills? School Psychology
Review, 46(2), 201–218.
Hughes, J. N., & Im, M. H. (2016). Teacher-Student Relationship and Peer Disliking and Liking
Across Grades 1-4. Child Development, 87(2), 593–611. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12477
Jerome, E. M., Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Teacher–child relationships from
kindergarten to sixth grade: Early childhood predictors of teacher‐perceived conflict and
closeness. Social Development, 18(4), 915–945.
Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., & Pelletier, M. E. (2008). Teachers’ views and beliefs about bullying:
Influences on classroom management strategies and students’ coping with peer
victimization. Journal of School Psychology, 46(4), 431–453.
Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. W. (1996). Peer victimization: Cause or consequence of school
maladjustment? Child Development, 67(4), 1305–1317.
Ladd, G. W. (1990). Having friends, keeping friends, making friends, and being liked by peers in
the classroom: Predictors of children’s early school adjustment? Child Development, 61(4),
1081–1100.
Ladd, G. W., Ettekal, I., & Kochenderfer-ladd, B. (2017). Peer victimization trajectories from
kindergarten through high school : Differential pathways for children ’ s school engagement
and achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(6), 1–16.
https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000177
Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C. (1996). Friendship quality as a predictor of
young children’s early school adjustment. Child Development, 67(3), 1103–1118.
Teacher Influences 26
Ogelman, H. G., Erol, A., Karaşahin, N., & Akyol, M. (2019). Fathers and Their Young
Children’s Self-Perception, Levels of Being Liked by Their Peers and Prosocial Behaviours.
International Journal of Academic Research in Education, 5(1), 31–42.
Önder, A., Gülay Ogelman, H., & Göktaş, İ. (2019). Examining the correlations between the
self-perception of preschool children and their levels of being liked by their peers. Early
Child Development and Care, 1–9.
Parkhurst, J. T., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Peer rejection in middle school: Subgroup differences in
behavior, loneliness, and interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 28(2), 231.
Perlman, M., Falenchuk, O., Fletcher, B., McMullen, E., Beyene, J., & Shah, P. S. (2016). A
systematic review and meta-analysis of a measure of staff/child interaction quality (the
classroom assessment scoring system) in early childhood education and care settings and
child outcomes. PloS One, 11(12), e0167660.
Pianta, R. C., La Paro, K. M., & Hamre, B. K. (2008). Classroom Assessment Scoring SystemTM:
Manual K-3. Paul H Brookes Publishing.
Quinn, M., & Hennessy, E. (2010). Peer relationships across the preschool to school transition.
Early Education and Development, 21(6), 825–842.
Troop-Gordon, W., MacDonald, A. P., & Corbitt-Hall, D. J. (2019). Children’s peer beliefs,
friendlessness, and friendship quality: Reciprocal influences and contributions to
internalizing symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 55(11), 2428–2439.
Wang, C., Hatzigianni, M., Shahaeian, A., Murray, E., & Harrison, L. J. (2016). The combined
effects of teacher-child and peer relationships on children’s social-emotional adjustment.
Journal of School Psychology, 59, 1–11.
Waters, S. K., Lester, L., Wenden, E., & Cross, D. (2012). A theoretically grounded exploration
Teacher Influences 27
of the social and emotional outcomes of transition to secondary school. Journal of
Psychologists and Counsellors in Schools, 22(2), 190–205.
Teacher Influences 28
Table 1
Sample description
Variable
Valid
N
%
missing
Mean, % SD Range
Child and family characteristics
School district 2678 0.0
District 1 62.1%
District 2 37.9%
Grade level 2678 0.0
Preschool 21.7%
Kindergarten 24.7%
First grade 17.8%
Second grade 18.9%
Third grade 16.9%
Child gender 2659 0.7
Female 49.5%
Male 50.5%
Child race 2628 1.9
White/Caucasian (non-multiracial) 66.1%
Black/African American (non-
multiracial)
8.0%
Asian (non-multiracial) 6.3%
Other (non-multiracial) 7.9%
Multiracial 11.8%
Child is Hispanic 2637 1.5 12.8%
Child has an IEP in spring 2450 8.5 10.1%
Primary language spoken at home is
English
2649 1.1 87.9%
Annual household income 2564 4.3
< $30,001 27.2%
$30,001 ~ $60,000 16.8%
$60,001 ~ $90,000 12.9%
$90,001 ~ $120,000 12.0%
> $120,000 31.1%
Mother’s highest level of education 2619 2.2
Less than high school diploma 10.3%
High school diploma or GED 31.8%
Associate degree 12.8%
Bachelor’s degree 24.8%
Graduate or professional degree 20.3%
Child age in fall (in months) 2650 1.0 78.16 18.37 25~124
Number of people in household 2026 24.3 4.51 1.23 2~9+
Number of children (age < 18) in
household
2026 24.3 2.47 1.12 1~9+
Note. Means are reported for continuous variables and percentages reported for categorical
variables.
Teacher Influences 29
Table 1 (Cont’d)
Sample description
Variable
Valid
N
%
missing
Mean, % SD Range
Classroom and teacher characteristics
School district 183 0.0
District 1 64.5%
District 2 35.5%
Grade level 183 0.0
Preschool 25.7%
Kindergarten 25.7%
First grade 15.8%
Second grade 16.9%
Third grade 15.8%
Teacher gender 178 2.7
Female 97.2%
Male 2.8%
Teacher race 175 4.4
White/Caucasian (non-multiracial) 96.0%
Black/African American (non-
multiracial)
2.3%
Other (non-multiracial) and
Multiracial
1.7%
Teacher ethnicity (1 =
Hispanic/Latino(a))
174 4.9 1.1%
Certification status (1 = Yes) 169 7.7 82.8%
Teacher’s highest level of education 174 4.9
High school diploma or GED 1.1%
Some college credit, no degree 2.3%
Associate degree 2.3%
Bachelor’s degree 35.1%
Master’s degree 59.2%
Teacher age (in years) 179 2.2 37.66 9.05 22~60
Teaching experience (in years) 173 5.5 13.39 8.15 2~36
Number of children in classroom 178 2.7 21.90 3.99 12~29
Note. Means are reported for continuous variables and percentages reported for categorical
variables.
Teacher Influences 30
Table 2
Descriptives of key study variables
Variable N % missing Mean SD Range
Child-perceived peer social experiences
Child-perceived peer support fall 2214 17.3 1.32 0.42 0.00~2.00
Child-perceived peer support spring 2443 8.8 1.35 0.39 0.00~2.00
Child-perceived peer victimization fall 2234 16.6 0.44 0.51 0.00~2.00
Child-perceived peer victimization spring 2457 8.3 0.53 0.54 0.00~2.00
Actual peer social interactions (standardized)
Reciprocal friends 2461 8.1 .08 .07 .00~.38
Reputation of peer victimization 2662 0.6 .04 .06 .00~.80
Teacher-child relationships
Teacher-child closeness 2293 14.4 3.13 0.69 0.00~4.00
Teacher-child conflict 2293 14.4 0.63 0.80 0.00~4.00
Classroom social management
CLASS behavior management 179 2.2 5.42 0.78 3.00~7.00
CLASS teacher sensitivity 179 2.2 4.65 1.02 2.00~7.00
CLASS positive climate 179 2.2 5.52 0.84 2.50~7.00
CLASS negative climate 179 2.2 6.92 0.24 5.50~7.00
Teacher Influence
Table 3
Pearson correlation coefficients among key study variables
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
Child-perceived peer social experiences
1. Peer support fall —
2. Peer support spring .55* —
3. Peer victimization fall .18 .06 —
4. Peer victimization spring -.13 .20* .41* —
Actual peer social interactions
5. Reciprocal friends .07 .10 -.11 -.12 —
6. Reputation of peer victimization -.07 .03 -.02 .11 -.13 —
Teacher-child relationship
7. Teacher-child closeness .16 .13 .04 -.09 .05 -.13 —
8. Teacher-child conflict -.18 -.12 .14 .16 -.01 .15 -.27* —
Classroom social management
9. CLASS behavior management -.07 -.14 -.25* -.20* .07 -.12 -.07 .12 —
10. CLASS teacher sensitivity -.07 .01 -.13 -.06 -.05 -.04 -.06 .07 .38* —
11. CLASS positive climate -.07 -.06 -.12 -.16* .08 -.08 -.10 -.07 .70* .51* —
12. CLASS negative climate .03 .07 -.15 -.11 .06 -.08 -.02 .05 .42* .17* .40* —
* p < .05.
Teacher Influence
Table 4
Predicting child-perceived peer social support and peer victimization in spring: Model 1
Peer social support Peer victimization
Coefficient SE Coefficient SE
Pretest
Child-perceived peer social support fall 0.34*** 0.02
Child-perceived peer victimization fall 0.37*** 0.02
Demographics
Preschool vs. K -0.00 0.03 -0.04 0.05
Grade 1/2/3 vs. K 0.03 0.03 -0.03 0.04
District 1 vs. 2 -0.01 0.02 0.07* 0.03
Child is a girl 0.01 0.02 0.04 0.02
Child has a disability (spring) -0.07* 0.03 0.02 0.03
Child is White 0.03 0.02 0.00 0.03
Teacher-child relationship and teacher practice
Teacher-child closeness (cmc) 0.04** 0.01 0.04 0.02
Teacher-child conflict (cmc) -0.02 0.01 0.10*** 0.02
CLASS behavior management 0.00 0.02 -0.07* 0.03
CLASS teacher sensitivity -0.00 0.01 0.01 0.02
CLASS positive climate 0.02 0.02 -0.00 0.03
CLASS negative climate 0.02 0.05 -0.07 0.07
Model information
AIC 1493.19 2564.03
BIC 1582.41 2653.41
Log Likelihood -730.60 -1266.02
Number of children 1951 1971
Number of classrooms 163 163
Level-2 variance (Intercept) 0.00 0.01
Level-1 variance (Residual) 0.12 0.20
Note: cmc: class-mean centered.
*** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05
Teacher Influence
Table 5
Predicting child-perceived peer social support and peer victimization in spring: Model 2
Peer social support Peer victimization
Coefficient SE Coefficient SE
Pretest
Child-perceived peer social support fall 0.33*** 0.02
Child-perceived peer victimization fall 0.37*** 0.02
Demographics
Preshool vs. K -0.01 0.03 -0.06 0.05
Grade 1/2/3 vs. K 0.01 0.02 -0.03 0.04
District 1 vs. 2 0.00 0.02 0.06* 0.03
Child is a girl 0.02 0.02 0.04 0.02
Child has a disability (spring) -0.06* 0.03 0.01 0.03
Child is white 0.02 0.02 0.00 0.03
Teacher-child relationship and teacher practice
Teacher-child closeness (cmc) 0.04** 0.01 0.04 0.02
Teacher-child conflict (cmc) -0.01 0.01 0.09*** 0.02
CLASS behavior management -0.00 0.02 -0.07* 0.03
CLASS teacher sensitivity 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.02
CLASS positive climate 0.02 0.02 0.01 0.03
CLASS negative climate 0.01 0.05 -0.07 0.07
Peer social interactions
Reciprocal friends 0.74*** 0.11 -0.09 0.15
Reputation of peer victimization -0.17 0.12 0.94*** 0.16
Model information
AIC 1436.71 2508.44
BIC 1536.86 2608.76
Log Likelihood -700.36 -1236.22
Number of children 1927 1946
Number of classrooms 163 163
Level-2 variance (Intercept) 0.00 0.01
Level-1 variance (Residual) 0.11 0.19
Note: cmc: class-mean centered.
*** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05